articol EPQ

download articol EPQ

of 5

Transcript of articol EPQ

  • 8/8/2019 articol EPQ

    1/9

     _______________________________________________________________  

     _______________________________________________________________ 

     

    Report Information from ProQuest

    June 18 2014 04:59

     _______________________________________________________________  

    18 June 2014 ProQuest

  • 8/8/2019 articol EPQ

    2/9

      able of contents

    1. The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Brief Version: Factor Structure and Reliability.............................. 1

    18 June 2014 ii ProQuest

  • 8/8/2019 articol EPQ

    3/9

    Document 1 of 1

     

    The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Brief Version Factor Structure and Reliability

    Author Sato, Toru 

    ProQuest document link 

    Abstract The short scale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised (EPQR-S; H. J. Eysenck &S. B. G.

    Eysenck, 1992) is a 48-item personality questionnaire primarily designed to measure an individual's level of 

    extraversion (vs. introversion) and neuroticism. Although L. J. Francis, L. B. Brown, and R. Philipchalk (1992)

    created the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Revised-Abbreviated (EPQR-A), an even briefer version of the

    EPQR-S, the reliability coefficients of some of the measures have been less than satisfactory (S. Forrest, C. A.

    Lewis, &M. Shevlin, 2000). Because brevity and reliability are both extremely important, the author of the

    present study created a briefer version of the EPQR-S, more reliable than the EPQR-A, by making slight

    alterations in the item content as well as the response format of the EPQR-S. Two hundred and sixty eight

    participants completed the original EPQR-S and the 24-item newly revised briefer version of the EPQR-S (EPQ-

    BV) twice. The findings revealed that the EPQ-BV has good internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and

    concurrent validity. A principal component analysis revealed a solution with factor loadings that accurately

    reflected the primary measures of the EPQR-S. These findings are discussed in relation to the psychometric

    properties of the EPQR-A and the original version of the EPQR-S. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT]

    Full text Headnote

     ABSTRACT.

    The short scale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised (EPQR-S; H. J. Eysenck &S. B. G. Eysenck,

    1992) is a 48-item personality questionnaire primarily designed to measure an individual's level of extraversion

    (vs. introversion) and neuroticism. Although L. J. Francis, L. B. Brown, and R. Philipchalk (1992) created the

    Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Revised-Abbreviated (EPQR-A), an even briefer version of the EPQR-S, the

    reliability coefficients of some of the measures have been less than satisfactory (S. Forrest, C. A. Lewis, &M.

    Shevlin, 2000). Because brevity and reliability are both extremely important, the author of the present study

    created a briefer version of the EPQR-S, more reliable than the EPQR-A, by making slight alterations in the item

    content as well as the response format of the EPQR-S. Two hundred and sixty eight participants completed the

    original EPQR-S and the 24-item newly revised briefer version of the EPQR-S (EPQ-BV) twice. The findings

    revealed that the EPQ-BV has good internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and concurrent validity. A

    principal component analysis revealed a solution with factor loadings that accurately reflected the primary

    measures of the EPQR-S. These findings are discussed in relation to the psychometric properties of the EPQR-

     A and the original version of the EPQR-S.

    Key words: assessment, introversion-extraversion, neuroticism, personality

    THERE ARE THREE CENTRAL "SUPERTRAITS" essential in understanding individual differences in

    personality, according to Eysenck (1990). These personality traits are extraversion (vs. introversion),

    neuroticism, and psychoticism. Extraverted individuals naturally have a lower arousal level than do introverts,

    which causes extraverted individuals to seek stimulation to raise their arousal level. In contrast, the naturally

    high arousal level of introverted individuals causes them to avoid stimulation as much as possible. This is why

    introverts tend to like quiet activities whereas extraverts tend to like stimulating activities (Eysenck, 1990).

    Individuals high in neuroticism tend to have a highly reactive autonomie nervous system, making them

    emotionally unstable. In contrast, the autonomie nervous systems of individuals who are low in neuroticism arenot very reactive, and they are therefore more emotionally stable (Eysenck, 1990). Individuals high in

    psychoticism tend to disregard common sense and behave impulsively (Eysenck, 1990). However, although

    psychoticism is considered to be the third trait in Eysenck's theory, most interest in this work has focused on

    18 June 2014 Page 1 of 7 ProQuest

    http://search.proquest.com/docview/213831522?accountid=15859http://search.proquest.com/docview/213831522?accountid=15859

  • 8/8/2019 articol EPQ

    4/9

    extraversion and neuroticism (e.g., Fink &Neubauer, 2004; Geen, 1984; Ramirez-Maestre, Martinez,

    &Zarazaga, 2004).

    For many years, researchers have continued to carefully develop questionnaires designed to measure

    individuals on these three personality traits (Eysenck, 1952, 1959; Eysenck &Eysenck, 1964, 1975, 1992;

    Francis, Brown, &Philipchalk, 1992). These researchers extended their efforts as far as embedding a lie Scale

    into the questionnaire. Since the creation of the original version, many efforts have been made to revise and

    improve the questionnaire (Eysenck, 1952, 1959; Eysenck &Eysenck, 1964, 1975, 1992). These revisions have

    typically caused the inclusion of increasingly more items, so that the questionnaire takes longer to administer.

     As a response to this trend, recent researchers have attempted to create briefer versions for practical purposes

    (Eysenck &Eysenck, 1975, 1992; Francis et al., 1992). One of the briefer versions is known as the Eysenck

    Personality Questionnaire-Revised Short form (EPQR-S; Eysenck &Eysenck, 1992). The EPQR-S is a 48-item

    personality questionnaire primarily designed to measure an individual's level of extraversion (vs. introversion),

    neuroticism, and psychoticism. Francis et al. later created the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Revised-

     Abbreviated (EPQR-A), an even briefer version of the EPQR-S. Although the brevity of this scale makes it very

    useful, some researchers have found that the reliability coefficients of the measures (especially the

    Psychoticism and lie scales) have been less than satisfactory (Forrest et al., 2000; Shevlin, Bailey, &Adamson,

    2002).

    Furthermore, because the original version was created in Great Britain, some of the items in the EPQR-S are

    more suitable for a British population than for an American population. Because this measure is also

    administered in the United States, it seemed useful to reword some of these items so that the EPQR-S would

    be suitable for both American and British populations. And because brevity, reliability, and item content are all

    extremely important factors in the development of a questionnaire, the goal of the present research project was

    to create a briefer version of the EPQR-S that would be both suitable to an American population and more

    reliable than the EPQR-A.

    To create this new briefer version (EPQ-BV), I made a number of adjustments (Sato, 2004). First, the

    psychoticism measure, which is both rarely used (e.g.. Fink &Neubauer, 2004; Geen, 1984; Ramirez-Maestre et

    al., 2004) and is associated with various psychometric problems (Ferrando, 2003; Forrest et al., 2000), was

    removed from the scale. Furthermore, because the lie Scale can be replaced by embedding items from other 

    scales measuring social desirability if necessary (e.g., Crowne &Marlowe, 1960), this scale was also eliminated

    for the sake of brevity. In addition, the response format was changed from a yes-no response to a 5-point Likert-

    type scale to increase the reliability of the measures. Last, because one of the items on the extraversion

    measure included an expression rarely used in the United States, it was slightly reworded. With the exception of 

    this wording change, the remaining items of the EPQ-BV were identical to the Extraversion and Neuroticism

    scales of the original EPQR-S (Eysenck &Eysenck, 1992). The purpose of the present study was to examinethe concurrent validity, internal consistency, and test-retest reliability of the EPQ-BV.

    Method

    Participants

     A total of 309 (129 men, 177 women, 3 unspecified) undergraduate university students participated in this study.

    Of the original 309 participants, I obtained data that was complete and suitable for analysis from 268 (98 men,

    168 women, 2 unspecified) of them (see procedure section for details). The mean age of the 268 participants

    was 19.4 years.

    Measures

    The EPQR-S form is a questionnaire that consists of three measures corresponding to the three personality

    traits in Eysenck's (1990) theory plus a lie scale (Eysenck &Eysenck, 1992). Each of the four measures

    contains 12 items. There are 2 reversed items in the extraversion measure, 7 reversed items in the

    psychoticism measure, and 9 reversed items in the lie Scale. There are no reversed items in the neuroticism

    18 June 2014 Page 2 of 7 ProQuest

  • 8/8/2019 articol EPQ

    5/9

    measure. The response format for all items is dichotomous (yes or no). With the exception of the psychoticism

    measure, all measures have good internal consistency and test-retest reliability (see Eysenck &Eysenck, 1992).

     

    The EPQ-BV is a newly revised version of the EPQR-S to measure individuals on two primary personality traits

    in Eysenck's (1990) theory. It consists of two measures, one for extraversion and one for neuroticism. The

    psychoticism and lie measures are not included in the EPQ-BV.

    In addition to these changes, to increase internal consistency, I changed the response format of the EPQ-BV

    from a yes-no format to a 5-point Likert-type scale with responses ranging from not at all (1), slightly (2),

    moderately (3), very much (4), to extremely (5). Last, because one of the items for the extraversion measure

    included an expression rarely used in the United States, it was slightly reworded. The word "bustle" in the item

    "Do you like plenty of bustle and excitement around you?" is a term rarely used in United States. Therefore, this

    item was changed to "Do you like plenty of action and excitement around you?" With the exception of this

    wording change, the remaining items are identical to the Extraversion and Neuroticism scales of the original

    EPQR-S (Eysenck &Eysenck, 1992). The final version of the EPQ-BV consists of 24 items (12 extraversion and

    12 neuroticism).

    Procedure

    The entire data collection process continued for 5 weeks. The questionnaires were completed in three sessions

    separated by 2 weeks between each session. In the first session, participants were randomly divided into two

    groups. In the first group, 153 undergraduate university students completed the EPQR-S. In the second group,

    156 undergraduate university students completed the EPQ-BV. Participants in both groups signed a consent

    form prior to completing the questionnaire in the first session. In the second session, 143 students who had

    completed the EPQR-S in the first session completed the EPQ-BV, and 147 students who completed the EPQ-

    BV in the first session completed the EPQR-S. In the third and final session, 271 of the participants who had

    completed the questionnaires in both previous sessions completed the EPQ-BV once again. All the participants

    were fully debriefed after the third session. Of the 271 participants with complete data, 3 scored 6 or higher on

    the lie Scale of the EPQR-S. The data of these participants were omitted in all subsequent analyses.

    Results

     An examination of the order effect of the questionnaires administered in the first two sessions (using I tests) did

    not yield any significant effects. The coefficient alphas for the Extraversion, Neuroticism, Psychoticism, and lie

    scales in the EPQR-S were .80, .78, 59, and .73, respectively. The coefficient alphas for the Extraversion and

    Neuroticism scales in the EPQ-BV were .92 and .90, respectively. The test-retest reliability values of the two

    scales of the EPQ-BV used in the present study were .92 for both the extraversion and neuroticism measures.

    Consistent with most research concerning the extraversion and neuroticism measures (e.g., Eysenck, Eysenck,

    &Barrett, 1985; Francis, 1993), t tests revealed that there were no significant gender differences on theExtraversion Scale, but women scored higher than men on the Neuroticism Scale on both the EPQR-S, t(262) =

    4.08, p

  • 8/8/2019 articol EPQ

    6/9

    I conducted a principal components analysis on all items in the EPQ-BV administered in the third session. An

    examination of the scree plot of eigenvalues revealed that the curve leveled off after the first two factors. These

    two factors (both with eigenvalues above 4) were retained, and I then conducted an oblique rotation factor 

    analysis restricted to two factors. The two factors combined accounted for approximately 51.8% of the total

    variance. Factor loadings generated by these analyses are presented in Table 2. All of the 24 items loaded

    above .48 on one of the two factors.

    The first factor accounted for approximately 31.4% of the variance. All 12 items loading on this factor were the

    items intended to measure extraversion. The factor loadings for all of the items in this factor were .60 or above

    (see Table 2). As expected, all items for the neuroticism measure loaded lower than .30 on this first factor.

    Furthermore, the reworded extraversion item, "Do you like plenty of action and excitement around you?" also

    loaded highly on this factor (factor loading of .73). The second factor accounted for approximately 20.4% of the

    variance. It consisted of all 12 items intended to measure neuroticism. Item loadings for all the items in this

    factor were .48 or above (see Table 2). As expected, all items for the extraversion measure loaded lower than.30 on this second factor.

    Discussion

    In the present study, the coefficient alphas for the Extraversion and Neuroticism scales in the EPQ-BV were

    higher than those of the original EPQR-S. These values were also higher than values reported for the EPQR-S

    and the EPQR-A in the past (Eysenck &Eysenck, 1992; Francis et al., 1992). The coefficient alphas for the

    Extraversion and Neuroticism scales of the EPQR-S are typically in the .78 to .87 and .79 to .83 range,

    respectively (Eysenck &Eysenck, 1992; Francis et al., 1992). Depending on the population sampled, the

    coefficient alphas for the extraversion and neuroticism measures of the EPQR-A are .74 to .84 and .73 to .77,

    respectively (Francis et al., 1992). The test-retest reliability values of the two scales of the EPQ-BV (.92) are

    comparable to other measures of extraversion and neuroticism such as the EPQR-S (Eysenck &Eysenck,

    1992).

    18 June 2014 Page 4 of 7 ProQuest

  • 8/8/2019 articol EPQ

    7/9

    The Extraversion Scale of the EPQ-BV was correlated highly with the extraversion measure in the original

    EPQR-S. Likewise, the Neuroticism Scale of the EPQ-BV was correlated highly with the neuroticism measure in

    the original EPQR-S. These correlations are comparable to the correlations reported with the measures in the

    EPQR-A by Francis et al. (1992). in which the correlations between the EPQR-S and the EPQR-A for 

    extraversion and neuroticism ranged between .92 and .95 and between .92 and .94, respectively. Perhaps the

    2- to 4-week time lag in between the two questionnaires contributed to the slightly lower correlations in my

    present study.

    In the present study, the EPQ-BV correlations between extraversion and neuroticism were comparable to the

    correlations typically reported with the measures in the EPQR-S and the EPQR-A. Although there has been

    much variability, the correlations between extraversion and neuroticism typically range between -.01 and -.37

    (Aluja, Garcia, &Garcia, 2003; Eysenck &Eysenck, 1992; Francis et al., 1992; Shevlin et al., 2002).

    To examine its factor structure, I conducted a principal components analysis on the EPQ-BV The results

    revealed a solution with factor loadings accurately reflecting the primary measures of the EPQR-S (extraversion

    and neuroticism). Although further replication using wider populations is necessary, the EPQ-BV, which takes

    less than 5 minutes to complete, seems to have high internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and a relatively

    robust factor structure. The next step for these new measures is to examine their convergent and divergent

    validity. For instance, examining individuals who score high and low on the extraversion measure in response to

    varying levels of sensory stimulation may be useful in examining the convergent validity of that measure.

    Overall, despite the fact the further examination of the EPQ-BV is necessary, this briefer version consisting of 

    the most commonly used measures of the original EPQR-S (extraversion &neuroticism) with only half of the

    number of items may serve as a suitable alternative for use in elaborate research that consists of additionalexperimental and time-consuming procedures.

    References

    18 June 2014 Page 5 of 7 ProQuest

  • 8/8/2019 articol EPQ

    8/9

    REFERENCES

     Aluja, A., Garcia, O., &Garcia, L. (2003). A psychometric analysis of the revised Eysenck Personality

    Questionnaire short scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 449-460.

    Crowne, D. P., &Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal

    of Consulting Psychology, 24, 355-360.

    Eysenck, H. J. (1952). The scientific study of personality. London: Routledge.

    Eysenck, H. J. (1959). Manual of the Maudsley Personality Inventory. London: University of London Press.

    Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality:

    Theory and research (pp. 244-276). New York: Guilford Press.

    Eysenck, H. J., &Eysenck, S. B. G. (1964). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. London:

    University of London Press.

    Eysenck, H. J., &Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. London: Hodder 

    &Stoughton.

    Eysenck, H. J., &Eysenck. S. B. G. (1992). Manual for the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised. San

    Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.

    Eysenck, S. B. G., Eysenck, H. J., &Barrett, P. (1985). A revised version of the Psychoticism Scale. Personality

    and Individual Differences, 6, 21-29.

    Ferrando, P. J. (2003). The accuracy of the E, N, and P trait estimates: An empirical study using the EPQ-R.

    Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 665-679.

    Fink, A., &Neubauer, A. C. (2004). Extraversion and cortical activation: Effects of task complexity. Personality

    and Individual Differences, 36, 333-347.

    Forrest, S., Lewis. C. A., &Shevlin, M. (2000). Examining the factor structure and differential functioning of the

    Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Revised-Abbreviated. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 579-588.

    Francis, L. J. (1993). The dual nature of the Eysenckian neuroticism scales: A question of sex differences.

    Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 43-59.

    Francis, L. J., Brown, L. B., &Philipchalk, R. (1992). The development of an abbreviated form of the revised

    Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQR-A): Its use among students in England, Canada, the USA, and

     Australia. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 443-449.

    Geen, R. G. (1984). Preferred stimulation levels in introverts and extraverts: Effects on arousal and

    performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1303-1312.

    Ramirez-Maestre, C., Martinez, A. E. L., &Zarazaga, R. E. (2004). Personality characteristics as differential

    variables of the pain experience. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 27, 147-165.

    Sato, T. (2004). The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire: A briefer and more reliable version. Paper presented

    at the 2004 annual convention of the Canadian Psychological Association, St. John's, Newfoundland, Canada.Shevlin, M., Bailey, F., &Adamson, G. (2002). Examining the factor structure and sources of differential

    functioning of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Revised-Abbreviated. Personality and Individual

    Differences, 32, 479-487.

    Original manuscript received July 8, 2004

    Final revision accepted December 28, 2004

    AuthorAffiliation

    TORU SATO

    Department of Psychology Shippensburg University

    AuthorAffiliation

     Address correspondence to Toru Sato, Department of Psychology, 213 Franklin Science Center, Shippensburg

    University, Shippensburg, PA 17257-2299; [email protected] (e-mail).

    18 June 2014 Page 6 of 7 ProQuest

  • 8/8/2019 articol EPQ

    9/9

    Subject Self evaluation; Psychology; Personality traits; Mental disorders; Questionnaires;

    MeSH  Adult, Factor Analysis, Statistical, Female, Humans, Male, Psychometrics, Reproducibility of Results,

    United States, Personality Inventory (major) 

    Publication title The Journal of Psychology 

    Volume 139

     

    Issue

    Pages 545-52 

    Number of pages 8 

    Publication year 2005 

    Publication date Nov 2005 

    Year 2005 

    Publisher Taylor & Francis Inc.

     

    Place of publication Provincetown 

    Country of publication United States 

    Publication subject Psychology 

    ISSN 00223980 

    CODEN

    JOPSAM 

    Source type Scholarly Journals 

    Language of publication English

     Document type PERIODICAL 

    Document feature Tables References 

    Accession number 16419443 

    ProQuest document ID

    213831522 

    Document URL http://search.proquest.com/docview/213831522?accountid=15859 

    Copyright Copyright HELDREF PUBLICATIONS Nov 2005 

    Last updated 2011-08-31 

    Database ProQuest Central 

     _______________________________________________________________ 

     Contact ProQuest 

    Copyright© 2014 ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. - Terms and Conditions 

    18 June 2014 Page 7 of 7 ProQuest

    http://search.proquest.com/docview/213831522?accountid=15859http://www.proquest.com/go/contactsupporthttp://www.proquest.com/go/contactsupporthttp://search.proquest.com/info/termsAndConditionshttp://search.proquest.com/info/termsAndConditionshttp://www.proquest.com/go/contactsupporthttp://search.proquest.com/docview/213831522?accountid=15859