accesare_resursa.pdf

download accesare_resursa.pdf

of 17

Transcript of accesare_resursa.pdf

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    1/17

    Multimodal Treatments for Childhood Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Interpreting Outcomes in the Context

    of Study Designs

    Betsy Hoza

    Nina M. Kaiser

    Elizabeth Hurt

    Published online: 21 August 2007

    Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

    Abstract The goal of this article was to outline issues

    critical to evaluating the literature on incremental benefit ofmultiple effective treatments used together, vs. a single

    effective treatment, for childhood ADHD. These issues

    include: (1) sequencing and dosage of treatments being

    combined and compared; (2) difficulty drawing valid

    conclusions about individual components of treatment

    when treatment packages are employed; (3) differing

    results emerging from measurement tools that purportedly

    measure the same domain; and (4) the resultant difficulty in

    reaching a summary conclusion when multiple outcome

    measures yielding conflicting results are used. The impli-

    cations of these issues for the design and conduct of future

    studies are discussed, and recommendations are made forfuture research.

    Keywords Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

    ADHD Multimodal treatment Combined treatment

    Behavior therapy

    In recent years, a number of multimodal treatment studies

    have emerged in the empirical treatment literature forchildhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

    These studies are important both for the level of design

    sophistication they contribute to the literature on ADHD

    and also because their results have influenced important

    documents, such as pediatric treatment guidelines and

    insurance coverage policies. The importance of these

    studies is indisputable, as is their impact on the way in

    which pediatricians, mental health professionals, schools,

    and parents approach treatment of childhood ADHD. We

    propose, however, that greater knowledge can be gained

    from these important studies by evaluating their outcomes

    in the context of the studies designs. This article repre-sents an attempt to focus greater attention on this issue.

    Our view is that no single study design can address all

    issues pertinent to treatment of a specific disorder. Indeed,

    there is no penultimate design, but rather, different designs

    address specific issues in a better or worse fashion. Hence,

    results of studies may be most informative to the field when

    interpreted in the context of their designs and greatest

    weight given to the questions best addressed by each

    design. Our purposes in writing this article are first, to

    illustrate the differential conclusions that may be reached

    as a function of whether design factors are emphasized in

    interpreting outcomes and second, to outline key issues forfuture research.

    In this article, we review multimodal treatment studies

    published roughly in the past decade (1995 to the present).

    We choose this time frame in order to restrict our studies to

    those using diagnostic criteria from the DSM-IV (Diag-

    nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth

    Edition; American Psychiatric Association 1994). This

    strategy allows us to eliminate confounds that may arise,

    when considering varying definitions of ADHD across

    Present Address:

    B. Hoza (&)

    E. HurtDepartment of Psychology, University of Vermont,

    2 Colchester Ave., Burlington, VT 05405-0134, USA

    e-mail: [email protected]

    N. M. Kaiser

    Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San

    Francisco, 401 Parnassus Ave., CPT, San Francisco,

    CA 94143-0984, USA

    e-mail: [email protected]

    E. Hurt

    Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA

    1 3

    Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334

    DOI 10.1007/s10567-007-0025-5

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    2/17

    multiple versions of the DSM. Within this time frame, we

    discuss issues pertaining to the way in which the studies

    designs may have affected their outcomes and conclusions.

    For purposes of this review, we limit our discussion to

    multimodal treatment studies examining incremental ben-

    efit, a subset of which have the added strength of also being

    multisite studies (Abikoff et al. 2004a,b; Hechtman et al.

    2004a,b; Hoza et al.2005; MTA Cooperative Group1999;Pelham et al. 2000; Wells et al. 2000, 2006). As in our

    prior work, we define incremental benefit studies as those

    wherein two distinct forms of active intervention [were]

    included (e.g., medication, behavior modification) and the

    use of one active treatment [was] compared to the use of

    two or more active treatments (Hoza et al. in press, p. 6).

    Of course the need for incremental benefit studies

    implies that single modality treatments for ADHD are

    insufficient to yield desired outcomes, a premise that

    applies better to some domains than to others. For example,

    whereas medication often controls core symptoms of

    ADHD reasonably well, when used alone, non-symptomdomains, such as parentchild relationships, peer relation-

    ships, aggression, and internalizing symptoms may fare

    better with a multimodal approach (MTA Cooperative

    Group1999). Furthermore, neither medication nor behav-

    ior therapy are effective for all children (Hoza et al.1999),

    and, even in children who may be responders to these

    treatments, non-treatment factors play a role in whether or

    not either treatment is utilized (Hoza et al. 2006). For

    instance, parental concerns regarding safety and accept-

    ability of medication use may interfere with initial

    implementation of treatment or sustained treatment

    adherence (Hoza et al.2006). Importantly, after decades of

    debate, recent evidence documents that ongoing use of

    stimulant medications may reduce childrens growth for up

    to three years (Swanson et al. 2007) and recent concerns

    have been raised based on animal research regarding the

    long-term effects of stimulants on the developing brain

    (Volkow and Insel2003). Similarly, behavioral treatments

    have drawbacks with criticisms centering on difficulty of

    implementation (Mrug et al.2001) and lack of evidence for

    generalization outside the treatment setting (Richters et al.

    1995). These limitations of pharmacological and behav-

    ioral treatments, however, can be viewed as indicating an

    even greater need for consideration of incremental benefit,

    as combined approaches often permit lower doses of

    medications and less complex behavioral strategies to be

    used (Pelham and Hinshaw 1992). For these reasons, we

    choose to focus herein on incremental benefit.

    Aside from this key requirement of considering incre-

    mental benefit, other requirements for inclusion in this

    review, also consistent with Hoza et al. (in press), were:

    First, studies were selected if investigators applied DSM-

    IV diagnostic criteria for ADHD, using either DSM-IV-

    based clinical assessments or diagnostic cutoffs on parent

    and/or teacher-completed dimensional (i.e., rating) mea-

    sures of ADHD symptoms. Importantly, prior work

    examining comparability of samples selected according to

    these two approaches provide evidence that they are rea-

    sonably comparable (Owens and Hoza 2003), although

    impairment and age of onset criteria are not necessarily

    captured to the same degree by the latter approach. Second,researchers included a multimodal intervention, at least one

    component of which could be classified as a psychosocial

    treatment based on well-established behavioral principles.

    Third, researchers employed some type of control condi-

    tion (this control could be pill or psychological placebo, an

    alternative treatment, a wait-list or no-treatment control, or

    treatment reversal), although we focus on evaluation of

    incremental benefit rather than comparisons to these con-

    trols in the current article. Fourth, researchers examined

    outcomes in at least one of six outcome domains of primary

    interest to us: ADHD symptoms (assessed via ADHD

    symptom rating scales completed by adult informants orbehavior counts evaluating frequencies of behaviors that

    are DSM-IV ADHD symptoms); behavior (oppositional

    and aggressive behavior, general behavioral or conduct

    problems); social/peer functioning (parent- and teacher-

    rated social function/dysfunction, peer nominations/ratings

    of friendship/liking/disliking, as well as adult evaluations

    of social skills), academic functioning, parenting practices/

    parentchild relationship measures, and child internalizing

    symptoms. Importantly, we include child self-report mea-

    sures only in those domains in which children generally are

    accepted as valid reporters (e.g., internalizing problems but

    not ADHD symptoms). Fifth, the participants were children

    of preschool- or school-age (with the goal of ensuring

    comparability of interventions across studies). Sixth, data

    necessary to compute effect sizes for post-treatment dif-

    ferences between treatment conditions were provided in the

    original article and/or contact information for the authors

    was available and the authors provided this information

    upon request.

    In the context of these parameters, we identified 13

    different studies (listed in Table1) described in 18 articles,

    as more than one article was published on some studies.

    Specifically, there were four articles based on the study by

    Abikoff and colleagues (hereafter collectively referred to

    as the Abikoff Group; Abikoff et al. 2004a, b; Hechtman

    et al. 2004a,b). There were three articles from the Multi-

    modal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (hereafter

    collectively referred to as the MTA Group; MTA Coop-

    erative Group1999; Wells et al.2000,2006). Ten of these

    studies employed between-subjects designs; two employed

    within-subjects designs, and one employed a single-subject

    design (see Table1for a listing of all studies according to

    these design classifications). In many instances, data were

    Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334 319

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    3/17

    Table1

    StudiesEmployingMultimodalInterventions

    A1:Multimodalinterventionstudie

    semployingbetween-groupsdesigns:symp

    tomandbehaviordomains

    Study

    N

    Diagnostic

    procedures

    **

    Treatment

    length

    Treatmentcomponents*

    Controls*

    Symptom

    measure**

    MeanES(range)

    Behavior

    measure**

    MeanES(range)

    Abikoff

    Group

    103s

    a,b,k

    24months

    1,2/4,6,8,10,11

    1

    c,d,k,l,m

    0.03(0.28,0.21)

    c,d,k,m

    0.03(0.39,0.45)

    1+21

    c,d,k,l,m

    0.29(0.53,0.00)

    c,d,k,m

    0.03(0.45,1.26)

    Barkleyetal.

    (2000)

    158s

    d

    12months

    2,6,7,11,1

    3,14,

    15,16,18

    2

    d,k,l

    0.18(0.12,0.41)

    d,i,k,l,m

    0.23(0.12,0.57)

    6,7,11,13,14,

    15,16,18

    d,k,l

    0.01(0.32,0.59)

    d,i,k,l,m

    0.05(0.22,0.37)

    24

    d,k,l

    0.04(0.16,0.28)

    d,i,k,l,m

    0.09(0.12,0.34)

    2

    6,7,11,13,14,

    15,16,18

    d,k,l

    0.21(0.90,0.59)

    d,i,k,l,m

    0.27(0.63,0.18)

    24

    d,k,l

    0.15(0.51,0.28)

    d,i,k,l,m

    0.14(0.36,0.13)

    6,7,11,13,

    14,

    15,16,18

    24

    d,k,l

    0.03(0.74,0.31)

    d,i,k,l,m

    0.13(0.16,0.47)

    Ercanetal.

    (2005)

    83s

    b,d,k

    6monthsmedication;

    eightsessions

    ofparenttraining

    1,2

    1

    d,k

    0.05(0.34,0.17)

    d,k

    0.05(0.15,0.26)

    Kleinand

    Abikoff

    (1997)

    89s

    b,k

    8weeks

    1,2,13,14

    1

    c,d,k,l,m

    0.38(0.10,0.99)

    c,d,k,l,m

    0.24(0.50,0.77)

    2,13,14

    c,d,k,l,m

    0.91(0.42,1.82)

    c,d,k,l,m

    0.82(0.00,2.14)

    2,13,14

    1

    c,d,k,l,m

    0.55(0.88,0.13)

    c,d,k,l,m

    0.55(1.13,0.00)

    MTA

    Coop.

    Group

    579s

    a,b,d,k

    14months

    1,2,9,12,1

    3,14

    1

    d,k,l

    0.30(1.47,0.15)

    d,i,k,l

    0.03(0.23,0.26)

    2,9,12,13,14

    d,k,l

    0.19(0.90,0.57)

    d,i,k,l

    0.29(0.07,0.48)

    22

    d,k,l

    0.18(0.74,0.71)

    d,i,k,l

    0.31(0.07,0.53)

    2,9,12,13,

    14

    1

    d,k,l

    0.47(0.61,0.38)

    d,i,k,l

    0.26(0.43,0.05)

    22

    d,k,l

    0.01(0.52,0.19)

    d,i,k,l

    0.02(0.25,0.21)

    Pelhametal.

    (2000)

    117s

    b,k

    14months

    1,2,9(6,11,15,16,18,

    25,26),12,13,14

    2,9

    j,k

    0.45(0.31,0.58)

    j,k,t

    0.21(0.03,0.72)

    Pfiffnerand

    McBurnett

    (1997)

    27s

    a,b,d

    8wks

    2,6(15,19)

    ,14

    6

    NA

    d,k

    0.04(0.04,0.11)

    20

    d,k

    0.99(0.42,1.56)

    6

    20

    d,k

    1.07(0.60,1.53)

    Tuttyetal.

    (2003)

    100s

    a,b,d,k

    8weeklysessions

    1,2,6,7

    1,22

    d,k

    0.44(0.11,0.77)

    NA

    vanderOord

    etal.(2007)

    45s

    b

    10weeks

    1,2,6/7/10,

    14

    1

    d,k

    0.10(0.22,0.44)

    d,k

    0.24(0.06,0.58)

    320 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    4/17

    Table1

    continued

    A2:Multimodalinterventionstudie

    semployingwithin-groupsdesigns:sympto

    mandbehaviordomains

    Study

    N

    Diagnostic

    procedures*

    *

    Treatment

    length

    Treatmentcomponents*

    Controls*

    Symptom

    measure

    **

    MeanES

    (range)

    Behavior

    measure**

    MeanES(range)

    Kolkoetal.

    (1999)

    16s

    a,b,d,k

    8weeks;b-modevery

    otherwk,2med

    dosesadminrandomly

    1x/wkduringwks38

    1,9(15,16,18,25)

    1

    j

    0.05(0.23,0.33)j,t

    0.13(0.41,0.72)

    9

    j

    0.51(0.22,0.79)

    j,t

    0.27(0.26,0.85)

    23

    j

    1.92(1.72,2.12)

    j,t

    0.77(0.41,1.47)

    9

    1

    j

    0.44(0.98,0.10)j,t

    0.13(0.69,0.68)

    23

    j

    1.14(0.56,1.71)

    j,t

    0.50(0.47,1.09)

    Pelhametal.

    (2005a)

    21s

    a,b,d,k

    8weeks;b-modevery

    otherweekfor4wks;

    3meddoseseach

    randomlyadmin

    1x/wkfor6wks

    1,9(2,14,15,16,

    18,25)

    1

    j,k

    0.93(0.70,1.15)

    j,k,t

    0.77(0.53,1.31)

    9

    j,k

    1.55(1.31,1.78)

    j,k,t

    1.19(0.72,1.86)

    23

    j,k

    3.02(1.89,4.15)

    j,k,t

    1.83(0.65,3.97)

    9

    1

    j,k

    0.61

    (0.65,0.56)

    j,k,t

    0.40(0.79,0.01)

    23

    j,k

    0.96 (0.63,1.29)

    j,k,t

    0.77 (0.25,1.8

    1)

    A3:Multimodalinterventionstudie

    semployingsinglecasedesigns:symptom

    andbehaviordomains

    Study

    N

    Diagnostic

    procedures**

    Treatmentlength

    Treatment

    components*

    Controls*

    Symptom

    measure**

    Effect

    Behavior

    measure*

    *

    Effect

    Reitman

    etal.

    (2001)

    3s

    a,b,d

    20days;3040

    observations

    1,15

    23

    m

    IM

    t

    IM

    1,15

    1

    m

    IM

    t

    IM

    1,15

    15

    m

    IM

    t

    IM

    15

    1

    m

    IM

    t

    IM

    15

    23

    m

    IM

    t

    IM

    1

    23

    m

    IM

    t

    IM

    B1:Multimodalinterventionstudie

    semployingbetween-groupsdesigns:parenting/parentchildrelationshipandinterna

    lizingdomains

    Study

    N

    Diagnostic

    procedures**

    Treatmentlength

    Treatment

    components*

    Controls*

    Parentin

    g

    measure**

    MeanES

    (range)

    Internalizing

    measure**

    MeanES

    (range)

    Abikoff

    Group

    103sa,b,k

    24months

    1,2/4,6,

    8,10,11

    1

    e,f

    0.12(0.35,0.25)s

    0.29

    1+21

    e,f

    0.31(0.57,0.13)s

    0.3

    Barkleyetal.

    (2000)

    158sd

    12months

    2,6,7,11,13,14,

    15,16,

    18

    2

    e,i

    0.09(0.03,0.29)

    d,k

    0.28(0.03,0.35)

    6,7,11,13,14,

    15,16,18

    e,i

    0.04(0.09,0.03)d,k

    0.07(0.39,0.10)

    24

    e,i

    0.03(0.09,0.19)d,k

    0.13(0.00,0.21)

    Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334 321

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    5/17

    Table1

    continued

    B1:Multimodalinterventionstudiesemployingbetween-groupsdesigns:parenting/parentchildrelationshipandinterna

    lizingdomains

    Study

    N

    Diagnostic

    procedures**

    Treatment

    length

    Treatment

    components*

    Controls*

    Symptom

    measure*

    *

    MeanES

    (range)

    Behavior

    measure**

    MeanES(range)

    2

    6,7,11,13,14,

    15,16,18

    e,i

    0.15(0.42,0.06)

    d,k

    0.35(0.50,0.20)

    24

    e,i

    0.06(0.10,0.02)

    d,k

    0.16(0.31,0.09)

    6,7,11,13,14,

    15,16,18

    24

    e,i

    0.09(0.02,0.30)

    d,k

    0.20(0.04,0.55)

    Ercanetal.

    (2005)

    83s

    b,d,k

    6monthsmedication;

    eightsessionsparent

    training

    1,2

    1

    g

    0.02

    NA

    Kleinand

    Abikoff

    (1997)

    89s

    b,k

    8weeks

    1,2,13,14

    1

    NA

    d,k

    0.10(0.28,0.32)

    2,13,14

    d,k

    0.09(0.57,0.20)

    2,13,14

    1

    d,k

    0.19(0.00,0.32)

    MTA

    Group

    579sa,b,d,k

    14months

    1,2,9,12,13,14

    1

    e,f,g,h,i

    0.22(0.01,0.42)

    d,k,s

    0.11(0.23,0.00)

    2,9,12,13,14

    e,f,g,h,i

    0.18(0.05,0.37)

    d,k,s

    0.03(0.24,0.26)

    22

    e,f,g,h,i

    0.18(0.07,0.50)

    d,k,s

    0.09(0.13,0.37)

    2,9,12,13,14

    1

    e,f,g,h,i

    0.05(0.15,0.12)

    d,k,s

    0.08(0.25,0.11)

    22

    e,f,g,h,i

    0.002(0.14,0.23)d,k,s

    0.13(0.00,0.26)

    Pfiffnerand

    McBurnett

    (1997)

    27s

    a,b,d

    8wks

    2,6(15,1

    9),14

    6

    NA

    d,k

    0.32(0.76,0.13)

    20

    d,k

    0.31(0.21,0.40)

    6

    20

    d,k

    0.67(0.26,1.08)

    Tuttyetal.

    (2003)

    100sa,b,d,k

    8weeklysessions

    1,2,6,7

    1,22

    e

    0.45

    NA

    vanderOord

    etal.(2007)

    45s

    b

    10weeks

    1,2,6/7/10,14

    1

    NA

    s

    0.03

    B2:Multimodalinterventionstudiesemployingwithin-groupsdesigns:parenting/parentchildrelationshipandinternalizingdomains:none

    B3:Multimodalinterventionstudiesemployingsinglecasedesigns:parenting

    /parentchildrelationshipandinternalizingdomains:none

    C1:Multimodalinterventionstudie

    semployingbetween-groupsdesigns:acad

    emicandsocial/peerdomains

    Study

    N

    Diagnostic

    procedures**

    Treatment

    length

    Treatment

    components*

    Controls*

    Academic

    measure**

    MeanES

    (range)

    Social/peer

    measure**

    MeanES

    (range)

    Abikoff

    Group

    103sa,b,k

    24months

    1,2/4,6,8

    ,10,11

    1

    d,q

    0.15(0.64,0.31)

    d,k,l,s

    0.01(0.31,0.42)

    1+21

    d,q

    0.18(0.82,0.15)

    d,k,l,s

    0.10(0.46,0.16)

    Barkleyetal.

    (2000)

    158sd

    12months

    2,6,7,11,13,14,

    15,16,18

    2

    k,q

    0.12(0.29,0.17)

    d,k

    0.48(0.20,0.71)

    322 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    6/17

    Table1

    continued

    C1:Multimodalinterventionstudiesemployingbetween-groupsdesigns:academicandsocial/peerdomains

    6,7,11,13,14,

    15,16,18

    k,q0.25(0.56,0.04)d,k

    0.17(0.31,0.02)

    24

    k,q0.08(0.37,0.17)

    d,k

    0.03(0.30,0.33)

    2

    6,7,11,13,14,15,16,18k,q0.12(0.42,0.04)

    d,k

    0.58(0.76,0.45)

    24

    k,q

    0.04(0.28,0.26)

    d,k

    0.42(0.48,0.34)

    6,7,11,13,14,15,16,1824

    k,q

    0.16(0.07,0.45)

    d,k

    0.18(0.04,0.45)

    Hozaetal.

    (2005)

    285sb,k

    14months

    1,2,9,12,13,14

    1

    NA

    r

    0.06(0.21,0.06)

    2,9,12,13,14

    r

    0.21(0.12,0.37)

    22

    r

    0.05(0.01,0.11)

    2,9,12,13,14

    1

    r

    0.27(0.41,0.08)

    22

    r

    0.17(0.27,0.09)

    Kleinand

    Abikoff

    (1997)

    89s

    b,k

    8weeks

    1,2,13,14

    1

    p,q0.16(0.27,0.08)c,d,k,m

    0.06(0.18,0.00)

    2,13,14

    p,q

    0.11(0.02,0.33)

    c,d,k,m

    0.18(0.72,0.96)

    2,13,14

    1

    p,q0.35(0.92,0.09)c,d,k,m

    0.23(0.91,0.50)

    MTAGroup

    579sa,b,d

    ,k14months

    1,2,9,12,13,14

    1

    q

    0.07(0.05,0.10)

    d,k,r

    0.09(0.05,0.19)

    2,9,12,13,14

    q

    0.15(0.01,0.23)

    d,k,r

    0.35(0.27,0.42)

    22

    q

    0.16(0.01,0.27)

    d,k,r

    0.30(0.18,0.46)

    2,9,12,13,14

    1

    q

    0.08(0.16,0.04)

    d,k,r

    0.25(0.40,0.08)

    22

    q

    0.00(0.04,0.05)

    d,k,r

    0.05(0.19,0.03)

    Pelhametal.

    (2000)

    117sb,k

    14months

    1,2,9(6,11,15,16,18,

    25,26),

    12,13,14

    2,9

    n,o

    0.15(0.09,0.25)

    j,r,t

    0.39(0.18,0.77)

    Pfiffnerand

    McBurnett(1997)

    27s

    a,b,d

    8wks

    2,6(15,19),14

    6

    NA

    d,k,s

    0.37(0.52,0.27)

    20

    d,k,s

    1.53(0.42,2.41)

    6

    20

    d,k,s

    2.11(0.68,3.41)

    vanderOord

    etal.

    (2007)

    45s

    b

    10weeks

    behavioral

    treatment

    1,2,6/7/10,14

    1

    NA

    d,k

    0.14(0.08,0.19)

    C2:Multimodalinterventionstudiesemployingwithin-groupsdesigns:academicandsocial/peerdomains

    Kolkoetal.(1999)

    16s

    a,b,d

    ,k8weeks;b-modevery

    otherwk,2meddoses

    adminrandomly1x/wk

    duringwks38

    1,9(15,16,18,25)

    1

    NA

    j

    0.15(0.37,0.66)

    9

    j

    0.46(0.39,0.53)

    23

    j

    0.98(0.65,1.31)

    9

    1

    j

    0.13(0.56,0.31)

    23

    j

    0.60(0.21,0.98)

    Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334 323

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    7/17

    Table1

    continued

    C2:Multimodalinterventionstudie

    semployingwithin-groupsdesigns:academ

    icandsocial/peerdomains

    Pelhametal.(2005a)

    21s

    a,b,d,k

    8weeks;b-modeve

    ry

    otherweekfor4wks;

    3meddoseseach

    randomlyadmin

    1x/wkfor6wks

    1,9

    1

    n,o

    0.01(0.44,0.46)

    j,k

    0.35(0.48,0.22)

    9

    n,o

    1.10(0.71,1.49)

    j,k

    0.49(0.37,0.61)

    23

    n,o

    1.22(0.60,1.83)

    j,k

    2.32(1.11,3.53)

    9

    1

    n,o

    0.88(0.94,0.82)

    j,k

    0.76(0.99,0.52)

    23

    n,o

    0.15(0.05,0.35)

    j,k

    1.28(0.72,1.84)

    C3:Multimodalinterventionstudiesemployingsinglecasedesigns:academic

    andsocial/peerdomains:none

    Note:NA=notapplicable.UnderN:p

    =preschool;s

    =school-age.*Treatme

    ntcomponentsandcontrolconditions:1=

    stimulantmedicationormedicationassess

    ment/referral;2=parent

    training;3=parent-targetedpartnersupportand/orcopingskilltraining;4=familyproblem-solvingorcommunicationtherapy;5=supportgroup;6=child-targe

    tedsocialskillstraining;

    7=child-targetedself-control/angermanagementtraining;8=individualchild-targetedpsychotherapy;9=behavioralsum

    mertreatmentprogram;10=childfocused

    academicorganizational

    skillstraining;11=individualized

    academicassistance/remediation/tutoring;12=behaviorallytrainedindividualparapr

    ofessionalsupport;13=schoolconsultatio

    nand/orteacherpsycho-

    education;14=dailyreportcard;15=tokenreinforcement;16=tokenrespon

    se-cost;17=delayedreward;18=timeout;19=peerattention/feedback/positiverein

    forcement;20=wait-list

    control;21=attentioncontrol;22

    =communitycare;23=treatmentwithdrawal;24=nointervention;25=adultadm

    inisteredpositivereinforcement/praise;26=sportsskillstraining.

    **Diagnosticandoutcomemeasure

    s:a=clinicaldiagnosisbypediatricianormentalhealthprofessional;b=clinicianad

    ministeredinterviewwithparent,child,and

    /orteacher;c=clinician

    ratings;d=parentratings;e=parentratingsofparentingpractices/strategies;f

    =childratingsofparentingpractices/strategies;g=parentratingsofparentchildrelationship;h=childratings

    ofparentchildrelationship;i=parentchildobservations;j=counselorratin

    gs;k=teacherratings;l=classroomobse

    rvations;m=observerratings;n=academ

    icproductivity;o=aca-

    demicperformance;p=cognitivetests;q=achievementtests;r=peerratingsornominations;s=childself-ratings;t=

    behaviorcounts.Forcasestudies,IM=improved;NC=nochange;

    DT=deteriorated.Therangeofeffectsizesisdesignatedby:(lowerbound,upperbound)

    AbikoffGroupstudiesincluded:A

    bikoffetal.2004a,b;Hechtmanetal.2004a,b

    MTAGroupstudiesincluded:M

    TACooperativeGroup1999;Wellsetal.2000,2006

    Hozaetal.(2005)andPelhametal.(2000)areconsideredseparatelyfrom

    theMTAGroupstudiesbecausethesestudiesarebasedononlyasubsetofMTAparticipants

    324 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    8/17

    available in the articles to make other comparisons in

    addition to examining incremental benefit (e.g., compari-

    son of a multimodal or unimodal treatment to a no

    intervention group). Although not explicitly discussed here,

    these comparisons also are included in the table for the

    interested reader.

    In evaluating incremental benefit, we provide data

    regarding the magnitude of incremental benefit treatmenteffects to the best of our ability, given the constraints of

    original articles. Specifically, within each relevant outcome

    domain, we report Cohens deffect sizes that assess post-

    treatment differences between the multimodal treatment vs.

    the initial or baseline treatment condition. Importantly,

    because treatment packages often were used as either the

    baseline or incremental treatment, a single treatment con-

    dition could include multiple treatment components; in

    interpreting results from these studies, effects sizes repor-

    ted pertain to the effects of the packages as a whole and

    conclusions may not be drawn about the individual com-

    ponent interventions. In computing effect sizes for within-subjects studies, we averaged data across all repetitions of

    the same treatment condition. Further, and regardless of

    type of study design, we averaged effect sizes across all

    dependent measures within each outcome domain of any

    given study in order to generate a single, overall effect size

    for the treatment effect in that domain and study. We note

    that results for different individual measures within a single

    domain did not always produce consistent results; conse-

    quently, our averaged results do not necessarily produce

    the same conclusions as those reported by the original

    authors based on individual measures. Finally, the reader

    should note that we altered the direction of effect sizes with

    the goal of improving ease of comparison across outcome

    domains. Thus, positive effect sizes denote improved

    functioning on the part of the multimodal treatment group

    or condition (i.e., incremental benefit) relative to the uni-

    modal treatment group or condition (e.g., lower ADHD

    symptoms, better academic performance). Effect sizes

    were interpreted according to the guidelines of Cohen

    (1988), who categorized values below 0.20 as non-impor-

    tant, values from 0.20 to\0.50 as small in magnitude,

    values from 0.50 to\0.80 as moderate in magnitude,

    and values at or above 0.80 as large. Where we discuss

    case studies that employed visual inspection of graphs to

    draw conclusions, we limit our discussion to descriptive

    comments and report for each domain, whether improve-

    ment, deterioration, or no change was observed (in the

    absence of any better method with which to quantify these

    effects). We recognize the limited generalizability of

    results from single-subjects designs to the ADHD popula-

    tion at large, yet include them for purposes of

    comprehensiveness.

    Using these parameters to structure our discussion, we

    organize our review around five key issues that we view as

    particularly relevant in examining evidence for incremental

    benefit of treatment for childhood ADHD: (1) How does

    choice of baseline or initial treatment(s), and particularly

    initial treatment dosage, affect conclusions about incre-

    mental benefit?; (2) How do we interpret studies involving

    treatment packages?; (3) How does choice of outcomemeasure(s) affect conclusions drawn?; (4) How do we

    reach a summary conclusion when multiple outcome

    measures are used?; and (5) What are the implications of

    these issues for the design and conduct of future studies? In

    discussing these issues, due to space limitations, we choose

    specific exemplars from the literature that we find partic-

    ularly useful in illustrating our points rather than

    attempting a discussion of each and every study indexed in

    Table1. However, we include a listing of all studies in the

    table for the interested reader.

    Issue 1: How does Choice of Baseline or Initial

    Treatment, Timing of the Addition of Treatments, and/

    or their Dosages, Affect Conclusions about Incremental

    Benefit?

    This issue lies at the crux of attempts to evaluate incre-

    mental benefit of multiple treatments over the effects of

    an initial treatment or treatment package. Yet, this issue

    has received little attention in the literature. First,

    regarding choice of initial treatment, because there is a

    finite amount of improvement that can occur in treatment

    studies in certain domains, there may be an inverse

    relationship between the amount of variance accounted

    for by an initial treatment, and the remaining variance

    that can be accounted for by a second treatment (Cunn-

    ingham 1999). In such a circumstance, the initial

    treatment may produce a large improvement, whereas the

    added treatment produces a lesser effect or no significant

    effect. In domains where no known treatment comes close

    to normalizing functioning (e.g., peer relationships), this

    is less of a concern. Nonetheless, the timing of when the

    treatments are added relative to one another is very

    important, especially in domains in which one of the

    treatments does come close to normalizing functioning.

    There is greater likelihood of the second treatment

    showing improvement either if both treatments are started

    together, if the second treatment is added to a lower dose

    of the initial treatment, or if the outcome domain is one in

    which neither treatment alone comes close to normalizing

    functioning.

    Examining the studies in Table 1, we see several illus-

    trations of this point. For example, the Abikoff Group

    Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334 325

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    9/17

    (Abikoff et al. 2004a,b; Hechtman et al. 2004a,b) inclu-

    ded only medication responders in their study and titrated

    these children to their maximum tolerable effective dose of

    methylphenidate (up to 50 mg/day) prior to beginning the

    psychosocial intervention (Klein et al. 2004). These

    researchers found no incremental effects of the psychoso-

    cial intervention, as reflected in our averaged effect sizes,

    in any of the domains summarized in the table. Similarly,researchers studying a sample of Turkish children with

    ADHD (Ercan et al. 2005), did not begin parent training

    until children had been on medication for a full month,

    with upward medication dose adjustments occurring during

    this month (and subsequently); these researchers found no

    evidence of incremental benefit of parent training. In

    contrast, Klein and Abikoff (1997) began medication and

    psychosocial treatments concurrently and found modest

    evidence for incremental benefit of combined treatment

    (medication plus a behavioral intervention) over the effects

    of medication alone in a subset of domains. Hence, timing

    of the initial treatment relative to the second treatment maybe a contributing factor in a studys outcome.

    Perhaps even more illustrative of this point are results

    from Pelham et al. (2005a), who crossed placebo and three

    doses of methylphenidate (MPH) with two levels of

    behavior modification (present vs. absent) in their study

    design. Since means and standard deviations were reported

    for all conditions, we were able to examine evidence for

    incremental benefit of combined treatment (intensive

    summer treatment program plus medication) as a function

    of the dose of medication in both medication-only and

    combined treatment conditions (12.5, 25, 37.5 mg

    transdermal MPH). As Fig.1 depicts, higher doses of

    medication in the medication-only condition were related

    to lower incremental benefit of combined treatment relative

    to medication-only treatment (averaged across 13 outcome

    variables in the behavior domain); this was true regardless

    of medication dose in the combined treatment condition.

    More specifically, incremental benefit of combined treat-

    ment was greatest when combined treatment was compared

    to the lowest dose in the medication-only condition (Co-

    hens ds averaged across the 13 outcome measures ranged

    from 0.9 to 1.2). Incremental benefit of combined treatment

    was lowest when the dose of medication in the medication-

    only treatment condition was highest (averaged Cohens ds

    ranged from 0.2 to 0.8). Hence, it is critically important

    that issues such as these be routinely discussed in inter-

    preting the results of clinical trials.

    Thus far, we have discussed the incremental benefit of

    adding behavioral treatment to medication as being largely

    dependent on the dose of medication being employed when

    behavioral treatment is added. However, the reverse phe-

    nomenon holds as well, as Pelham et al. (2000) aptly

    demonstrated. Specifically, using a subset of subjects from

    the MTA study, Pelham and colleagues compared children

    receiving a maximum intensity multi-component

    behavioral intervention package in the context of a com-

    prehensive summer treatment program to those receiving

    this same intensive behavioral treatment package combined

    with medication. Importantly, they reported no incremental

    benefit resulting from also receiving medication on 30 of

    35 dependent variables reported, despite the fact that

    medication is an intervention of known efficacy for ADHD.

    This study, together with those summarized immediately

    above, nicely illustrates the potential impact of dosage

    of initial treatments, whether pharmacological or behav-

    ioral, on comparisons evaluating incremental benefit.

    Fortunately, discussions of the effects of treatment

    sequencing and dosages on outcomes are emerging in the

    literature with increasing frequency. For example, in regard

    to the primary analyses from the Multimodal Treatment

    Study of Children with ADHD (MTA; MTA Cooperative

    Group1999), Pelham (1999) argued that the superiority of

    Medication Management over Behavioral Treatment at

    14 months was attributable to differences in dose or

    intensity of treatments at the 14-month assessment point,

    specifically, intensive medication vs. faded (i.e., lower

    intensity) behavioral treatment. Similar arguments have

    been made to explain the limited differences reported

    between Combined Treatment and Medication Manage-

    ment at 14 months in the MTA study (Cunningham1999).

    Follow-up articles directly examining these issues using

    data from the MTA study, however, have yielded seem-

    ingly conflicting findings. As just noted, Pelham et al.

    (2000) argue that comparisons between a subgroup of

    MTA participants in an early phase of treatment who were

    receiving Behavior Therapy during its most active and

    0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    1

    1.2

    1.4

    MED 12.5 MED 25 MED 37.5

    Medication Dose in Medication Only Condition (in mg)

    EffectSizeInde

    xingIncrementalBenefit

    COMB 12.5

    COMB 25

    COMB 37.5

    Fig. 1 Cohensdeffect sizes (averaged across 13 outcome variables

    in the behavioral domain) indicating incremental benefit of multi-

    modal treatment relative to medication only (transdermal

    methylphenidate), according to medication dose, in both combined

    and medication-only conditions. (Data taken from: Pelham et al.

    (2005a) )

    326 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    10/17

    intensive phase (i.e., during the summer treatment pro-

    gram), as compared to children receiving Combined

    Treatment, yielded little incremental benefit of Combined

    Treatment over very intensive Behavior Therapy, arguing

    that behavior therapy is a sufficient treatment used alone if

    it is intensive enough. However, Arnold et al. (2004),

    compared the MTA groups a number of months later at the

    9 month assessment point, when subjects were no longerinvolved in the intensive summer program, but parent

    training and the school-based behavior therapy intervention

    were only moderately intensive, and found results conso-

    nant with the 14 month findingsspecifically, superiority

    of Medication Management over Behavior Therapy alone

    for symptom measures.

    How do we make sense of these results? Our view is

    that these results are not necessarily contradictory if

    viewed within the context of the dosages of medication

    and behavior therapy being administered, since each of

    these treatments can vary widely in dosage levels. How-

    ever, dosages of behavior therapy have not been widelyconsidered in the psychosocial treatment literature. Yet, it

    is well known that direct contingency management (such

    as was delivered in Pelham et al. 2000) is more intensive

    and generally more effective than clinical behavior ther-

    apy, or behavior modification delivered by other agents

    parents, teacherswho have been taught the skills by

    clinicians (Pelham et al. 1998). Hence, comparisons of

    behavior therapy to other treatments in the MTA should

    yield more favorable results for behavior therapy when

    maximum intensity direct contingency management is

    employed (as in Pelham et al. 2000) than when less

    intensive clinical behavior therapy is employed, albeit

    with the support of paraprofessional classroom aides (as

    in Arnold et al. 2004). This indeed is the pattern that was

    obtained.1

    From our perspective, it is not problematic that results

    diverge within the contexts of these differing treatment

    designs; indeed, as we just have demonstrated, these

    divergences generally can be reconciled through careful

    contemplation of methodological and design issues. How-

    ever, parents, teachers, health care professionals, and other

    consumers of the results may interpret and utilize the

    results without careful consideration of these importantdesign issues, as they may not be immediately apparent to

    some consumers, especially those with little or no research

    training. Investigators can assist in ameliorating this con-

    cern by emphasizing these factors in research reports; and

    striving through a variety of media (e.g., question and

    answer sheets for parents and teachers) to make results of

    complex studies accessible to audiences of all levels of

    methodological sophistication. This is a very important

    point to which we will return in the discussion.

    Issue 2: How do we Interpret Studies InvolvingTreatment Packages?

    Most of the studies listed in Table 1 involved packages

    of psychosocial treatments. For example, in the Abikoff

    Groups study (Abikoff et al. 2004a, b; Hechtman et al.

    2004a, b; Klein et al. 2004) the multi-component psycho-

    social treatments employed included parent training/family

    therapy, academic skills training, individual psychotherapy

    for the child, and social skills training, as part of a psy-

    chosocial treatment package. Similarly, Pelham et al.

    (2000) employed a summer treatment program intervention

    involving multiple components including an extensive

    point system, social skills training and other peer inter-

    ventions (cooperative tasks with peers, a friendship

    intervention), sports skills training, a classroom interven-

    tion modeled after a special education classroom, a daily

    report card, and parent training. Indeed, multi-component

    treatment packages have, to some extent, become the norm

    in psychosocial treatment for childhood ADHD, largely

    due to the absence of a single successful treatment that

    effectively normalizes the many deficits associated with

    ADHD. From an historical perspective, then, the emer-

    gence of treatment packages makes sense, and represents a

    logical response to a disorder comprising impairments in

    multiple domains across the lifespan. From a research

    perspective, however, available data do not permit ade-

    quate evaluation of the individual components of the

    packages employed. Whereas evaluating individual com-

    ponents was unnecessary to the stated purposes of these

    studies, and, as we noted earlier, no single study can

    address all pertinent issues, this presents a quandary for

    those trying to provide evidence-based, cost-effective ser-

    vices, as well as for researchers trying to design future

    1 Of course, intensive behavioral therapies, similarly to high doses of

    medication, have limitations. Specifically, aside from the limited

    availability and high cost of intensive, complex, direct contingency

    management programs (see Barkley 2000, for a discussion of this

    issue in regard to the MTA study), an added difficulty for

    psychosocial treatment generally is that gains are difficult to maintain

    outside the treatment setting (Richters et al. 1995). Indeed, general-

    ization of gains and persistence of effects following participation inan intensive behavioral treatment program (e.g., Pelham and Hoza

    1996; Pelham et al. 2005b) has not been adequately studied. In

    addition, follow-up studies into adolescence and young adulthood of

    children who participated in intensive behavioral treatments as

    children are rare, and have only recently begun to appear in the

    literature. Consistent with other long-term follow-up studies of

    ADHD samples (for a brief review, see Richters et al. 1995), these

    studies document continuing adjustment problems and risky behav-

    iors into adolescence and adulthood relative to controls (e.g., Flory

    et al.2006; Molina et al.2007; Thompson et al. 2007). This suggests

    that short-term intensive behavioral treatment, like medication, has

    limitations.

    Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334 327

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    11/17

    studies. It is simply unclear exactly which components of

    treatment are most critical to include, or in what manner

    critical components are most effectively delivered. Of

    course, one could easily argue that until a comprehensive

    treatment package that successfully manages the chronic,

    varied symptoms and impairments of ADHD across mul-

    tiple developmental periods is identified, a dismantling

    strategy may be premature. We recognize the value in sucha position, yet, wish to point out the need for such an

    approach at the time that our state of knowledge reaches a

    point where it becomes feasible.

    In a related fashion, another issue that arises in the

    context of discussing treatment packages is whether con-

    sumers of such treatments view receiving more

    components as necessarily more desirable than fewer

    components. Indeed, relatively little published work has

    examined consumer satisfaction with regard to ADHD

    treatments. As the number of components increases, so too

    does the implementation burden on parents, teachers, and

    others involved in the intervention, consequently perhapsdecreasing compliance with the interventions and limiting

    potential gains. Further, costs of these interventions also

    generally increase as intervention packages become more

    complex. Studies are needed to determine whether and at

    what point consumer satisfaction and effectiveness may

    begin to diminish as additional components are added or as

    treatments are made more complex.

    Interestingly, available consumer satisfaction data sug-

    gest that parents report greater satisfaction with treatments

    involving behavioral components than with those involving

    medication alone (Pelham et al. 2007). Teachers in this

    same study similarly indicated that they felt better equip-

    ped to address ADHD in their classrooms when behavioral

    treatments were used, as compared to medication man-

    agement alone (Pelham et al. 2007). These results are

    impressive given the greater demands of the behavioral

    intervention in terms of time required from parents and

    teachers, as well as the level of improvement that was

    obtained for families receiving medication management.

    Yet, the point at which satisfaction begins to drop as

    demands increase on participants is an unknown area in

    need of further study.

    Issue 3: How does Choice of Outcome Measure(s) Affect

    Conclusions Drawn?

    In performing this review, we were struck by a simple fact:

    different outcome measures purported to measure the same

    domain often yielded drastically different results for the

    same comparisons within a study. The reader is encouraged

    to peruse the ranges of effect sizes obtained in each domain

    for each study, as presented in Table 1. Often effect sizes

    within the same domain were of widely varying magnitude

    and frequently also in opposite directions. For example, in

    the Klein and Abikoff (1997) study, for the ADHD

    symptoms domain, effect sizes for comparisons of com-

    bined psychosocial and psychostimulant treatment to

    stimulant treatment alone ranged from .10 to +.99,

    depending on the measure. In the MTA study (MTA

    Cooperative Group1999), also for the symptoms domain,comparisons of combined treatment to behavior therapy

    alone were similarly variable, with effect sizes ranging

    from .90 to +.57. Of importance, this pattern was not

    unique to specific studies, but for the most part, charac-

    terized this group of studies overall. This simple fact was

    surprising to us in preparing this article and may not be

    evident to the field at large, as results are generally not

    grouped by domain across sources, as we have done. This

    indicates the possibility that our methods, rather than our

    purported constructs being measured, may be driving the

    sizes of our effects, and relatedly, that the treatment effects

    that we obtain reflect, to a non-trivial degree, our chosenmeasurement methods and analysis strategies.

    We had difficulty in discerning any pattern in the vari-

    ation of effect sizes. This task was complicated by

    methodological differences among studies, such as those

    already discussed, as well as by the fact that each different

    study generally employed different outcome measures and/

    or different raters; for example, summer program-based

    studies (e.g., Kolko et al.1999) report ratings from summer

    program counselors rather than classroom teachers. No

    single study employed all possible types of outcome vari-

    ables within any given domain, making comparisons of

    effect sizes generated by data collected from one type of

    rater vs. another quite difficult.

    Nonetheless, there indeed appeared to be considerable

    variance both among and within raters. More specifically,

    in studies examining incremental benefit of multimodal

    psychosocial and stimulant treatment upon ADHD symp-

    toms or behavior vs. either psychosocial or stimulant

    treatment alone, averaged effect sizes based on teacher

    ratings generally were somewhat larger than were averaged

    effect sizes based on parent ratings.

    Further, even effect sizes from the same rater within the

    same study and purportedly assessing the same construct

    (yet based on administration of different measures) pro-

    duced differing results. As an example, Klein and Abikoff

    (1997) administered three separate measures of parent-rated

    hyperactivity, providing a unique opportunity to compare

    results of multiple measures of parent-rated hyperactivity.

    These measures (and effect sizes indexing incremental

    benefit of combined psychosocial and psychostimulant

    treatment over psychostimulant treatment alone) were as

    follows: Parent Home Hyperactivity Scale (Cohens

    d = 0.62); Parent CPRS Hyperactivity subscale (Cohens

    328 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    12/17

    d= 0.24); and Mother Hillside Behavior Scale, Gross

    Motor Activity subscale (Cohensd= 0.12). Based on these

    results, it seems likely that differences in items or in

    wording among these measures contributed to the discrep-

    ancies in effect size. Had these researchers not employed

    this comprehensive multi-measure approach, results would

    have depended upon the specific measure chosen.

    As mentioned earlier, our strategy for dealing with thesewidely varying results even within the same domain was to

    average effect sizes across all measures within a domain to

    yield one overarching effect size. But, we remind the

    reader that using this strategy, our conclusions often dif-

    fered from those of the original authors who interpreted

    measures individually and may have weighted one measure

    more heavily over another. We do not advocate for one

    approach (e.g., averaging effect sizes) over another (e.g.,

    interpreting individual effect sizes). Instead, we caution

    consumers of the treatment literature that choice of out-

    come measure will heavily influenceboth size and possibly

    also direction of effects in treatment outcome studies.Hence, results cannot and should not be interpreted outside

    the context of these factors.

    Issue 4: How do we Reach a Summary Conclusion

    When Multiple Outcome Measures are Used?

    In light of the pressing issue just described, a question of

    enormous importance for intervention researchers becomes:

    How do we reach a summary conclusion when multiple

    outcome measures are used? This is a dicey issue because

    use of multiple outcome measures, though consistent with

    generally recommended multi-method, multi-informant

    approaches, opens the door for pre-selection by investiga-

    tors based on any of a variety of factors. These may include

    practical issues such as how quickly or easily one measure

    vs. another can be made ready for analysis, or likelihood of

    publication based on size or strength of obtained effects.

    Unfortunately, null findings remain hard to publish (Ro-

    senthal1979). This appears to be true even when studies are

    well-designed and adequately powered; we are hopeful that

    this is beginning to change, as it is just as important to know

    what doesnotwork as it is to know what doeswork. These

    larger issues fall beyond the scope of this article, but are

    raised merely as a backdrop for the ensuing discussion.

    In this era of evidence-based treatments, with both best

    practices guidelines and treatment manuals widely avail-

    able, one might assume that similar guidelines exist to

    guide us in choosing assessment instruments, both for

    selection of study participants and in reporting and inter-

    preting outcomes from treatment studies. Unfortunately,

    few guidelines exist either for researchers who are report-

    ing research results in scientific journals, or for consumers

    of scientific journals, to assist in deciding what to conclude

    under these often confusing circumstances. As noted

    recently by Mash and Hunsley (2005), even though evi-

    dence-based treatments have advanced significantly over

    the past decade, it generally is the case that evidence-based

    assessment has not progressed in the same manner in

    regards to most childhood disorders. Yet, conclusions

    regarding treatment outcomes only are as valid as theprocedures used to select study participants and the reli-

    ability and validity of treatment outcome measures

    employed. Hence, the status of evidence-based assessment

    bears directly on the level of evidence for evidence-based

    treatments (EBTs). For example, practices vary widely

    across studies in terms of whether parent informants alone,

    or parent and teacher informants together, are used to

    gather diagnostic information. Despite this variability in

    assessment practices, however, and as noted by Mash and

    Hunsley (2005): For the most part, treatment-focused task

    force statements and guidelines have been silent on the use

    of assessment in developing and evaluating EBTs, not-withstanding general recommendations to use a reliable

    and valid core assessment batteryrecommendations that

    are presented with little guidance as to what makes an

    assessment method or process reliable or valid in a par-

    ticular context or for a particular purpose (pp. 363364).

    This measurement quandary, however, is not necessarily

    cause to throw the metaphorical baby out with the bath

    water. Rather, this dilemma begs for the development of

    measurement models that are mindful of these challenges,

    even if completely eliminating these challenges may be too

    ambitious a goal at the present time. Nonetheless, a key

    first step would be to outline the most important domains to

    be assessed and the types of measures to be included in

    treatment outcome studies for ADHD. Measures should

    include not just those most easily obtained, such as parent

    and teacher ratings, as ratings such as these are more prone

    to various types of bias (particularly if the informants are

    not blind to treatment conditions). Instead, these measures

    are best balanced to a reasonable degree by more objective

    measurese.g., peer sociometrics, achievement testing,

    behavior countseven though more objective measures

    such as these often are more expensive to obtain in terms of

    time and resources. Finally, given our current knowledge

    that ADHD is a chronic disorder that emerges early in

    childhood and persists into adulthood (for a discussion of

    long-term prognosis, see Richters et al. 1995), the appli-

    cability of available measures, as well as their reliability

    and validity across key developmental periods, is a par-

    ticularly important consideration.

    In addition to developmental sensitivity, importantly, the

    inclusion of measurements thought to be sensitive to the

    change produced byeachtype of treatment being compared

    seems important to producing a balanced comparison among

    Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334 329

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    13/17

    treatments (Cunningham 1999). For example, whereas

    medication might be expected to have a large impact on

    symptom reduction, behavioral treatments such as parent

    training might be expected to have the largest impact on the

    parentchild relationship. If this is the case, results may be

    most informative when these diverse measures can be

    examined and presented simultaneously. Publishing data in

    the order in which it becomes available based on ease ofpreparation for analysis ought to be discouraged, in the

    interest of presenting the most balanced picture.

    In sum, measurement models need to be developed that

    guide researchers in the selection of measures and that

    promote consistent choice of domains and measurement

    strategies across studies and across developmental periods.

    Such models do not currently exist in the ADHD literature

    or in the child psychological treatment literature at large,

    but the field would benefit greatly from their development.

    Attempts to produce such models should go hand-in-hand

    with developments in evidence-based assessment and

    necessarily should proceed in tandem. Indeed, the fact thatevidence-based treatment has marched ahead despite lim-

    itations in our evidence-based assessments is a

    complicating factor, as evidence-based assessment is a

    necessary prerequisite for measuring outcomes of evi-

    dence-based treatments.

    Issue 5: What are the Implications of these Issues for

    the Design and Conduct of Future Studies?

    We began this article by indicating that outcomes of

    treatment studies may be at least partially a function of

    their designs. To this simple premise, we added the notion

    that it is impossible for any given study to single-handedly

    answer most key treatment questions. These are points that

    we wish to reiterate as we discuss the implications of past

    work for future studies. Our recommendations for future

    investigators conducting research in this area follow from

    each of our discussion points. Admittedly, a number of our

    recommendations are applicable to research design gener-

    ally and may not be unique to ADHD treatment outcome

    research. Nonetheless, we believe a specific discussion of

    how these points apply in this context is useful, particularly

    since few such discussions appear in the ADHD literature

    specifically, or the child treatment literature more

    generally.

    Recommendation 1

    Use care to insure that your choice of baseline or initial

    treatment, the timing of when additional treatments are

    added, and the dosages of treatments employed match your

    research question. In other words, be clear regarding

    whether your goal is to study the benefit of adding a second

    treatment to one that is already administered at full inten-

    sity and considered primary, or whether you wish to

    consider dosage effects of both treatments simultaneously

    in order to evaluate the best dosage combination of treat-

    ments from each modality; these are very different

    questions. When the course of action chosen is the former,be cautious not to draw conclusions about the latter as part

    of the same study. In other words, for example, if a child is

    titrated to their maximum effective dose of medication and

    behavior therapy is thereafter added as a secondary treat-

    ment, avoid drawing conclusions about the relative

    effectiveness of any other dosage combinations of these

    two treatments. For example, based on this design, you

    cannot rule out the possibility that the child might respond

    just as well to a high intensity behavioral intervention

    coupled with a minimal medication dose; such a compar-

    ison must be made directly.

    Recommendation 2

    Use care not to draw conclusions about which compo-

    nent(s) are responsible for the success or failure of an

    intervention when you employ multiple treatments used

    together as a package. Similarly, avoid concluding that

    each of the components of a successful intervention are

    necessary to create that successful outcome. In like fashion,

    you cannot conclude that each of the components of an

    unsuccessful intervention are not valuable, as you cannot

    rule out the possibility that the various treatments under-

    mined one another. In other words, empirical studies

    employing a treatment package do not provide an evidence

    base for the components of the treatment package. Hence,

    if you design a study involving a treatment package, use

    care that it is only the package about which you draw

    conclusions. All other conclusions require a different

    design.

    Recommendation 3

    This recommendation, derived from issues 3 and 4, has to

    do with choice of outcome measures and how to reach a

    summary conclusion involving multiple (and often con-

    flicting) outcome measures. We believe that this is the area

    in need of the greatest work, and with the greatest potential

    to shape the field. As previously noted (Pelham et al.

    2005c), studies comparing assessment strategies are badly

    needed and few have been conducted to date. We see a

    particularly great need for development of standardized

    approaches to assessing treatment outcome that are

    330 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    14/17

    applicable across diverse developmental periods (e.g., from

    preschool-age to adulthood). This is not to say that each

    study or each developmental period should employ the

    exact same measures, but rather, common principles

    regarding domains assessed and types of measures required

    for adequate coverage of a domain should guide the field,

    with the goal of increasing comparability across studies

    and across the lifespan. A logical approach might includecovering 45 key domains of functioning, and including

    several established sources of information for each. These

    measures might include both pragmatic measures such as

    parent and teacher ratings (despite limitations such as rater

    bias), as well as more objective strategies such as behavior

    counts, peer nominations, and achievement testing. Based

    on the wide range of effect sizes displayed in Table 1, even

    for measures that purportedly tap the same domain, we

    advise extreme caution in relying on a single measure or

    type of measure and suggest that researchers err on the side

    of comprehensiveness rather than parsimony.

    In making this recommendation, however, we note thatwe would hope that any future evidence-based and stan-

    dardized approach to outcome assessment does aim for

    brevity; as any researcher or clinician knows, administering

    fewer measures is logistically easier and more cost-effec-

    tive than is administering more. Indeed, an important

    recent review (Pelham et al. 2005c) summarizes the state

    of the field in regard to assessment of ADHD and identifies

    this as one of numerous pressing issues for future research.

    Summarizing their conclusions is beyond the scope of this

    article and, hence, the reader is referred directly to this

    article. Among their key recommendations, however, is an

    appeal for simplicity, parsimony, a focus on functional

    problems as opposed to symptoms, and a return to greater

    use of functional behavior analysis in both initial assess-

    ment and in evaluating outcomes. These authors also

    suggest the possibility of a briefer assessment approach to

    ADHD based on a combination of simple strategies that

    have heretofore been used in a variety of separate studies;

    for example, a small number of items rated by parents and

    teachers, behavior counts of an individualized target

    behavior, teacher records of academic work completion

    and accuracy, and whether or not a child has their school

    supplies in their desk (Pelham et al. 2005c). These

    researchers acknowledge: The kind of large-scale

    research needed to validate this approach to assessment has

    not yet been conducted, but clinicians can be confident

    based on extant research that it has some empirical sup-

    port (Pelham et al. 2005c, p. 468).

    We agree with Pelham et al. (2005c) recommendation

    that assessment of functional difficulties (as opposed to

    DSM-IV symptoms) is most appropriate for clinical

    intervention, and we similarly find their arguments for

    brevity in assessment quite compelling. Still, we believe

    that precisely the type of large-scale research to which

    Pelham et al. refer is a necessary prerequisite that should

    occur prior to adoption of this type of assessment strat-

    egy. Thus, we do not endorse (at least not at the present

    time) combining several simple assessment strategies,

    each with some supportive evidence, in lieu of the cur-

    rently accepted comprehensive assessment approach. In

    our view, taking this approach to assessment in treatmentoutcome research may be premature in light of our find-

    ings that choice of outcome measure can dramatically

    affect the results of between-treatment comparisons.

    Instead, we encourage researchers to pursue validation of

    measurement models comprised of carefully selected

    combinations of measures, and to directly compare dif-

    fering combinations of measures in terms of their

    usefulness, reliability and validity, incremental predictive

    utility, applicability across diverse developmental periods,

    sensitivity to treatment response, and cost. In other words,

    we recommend an approach similar to our recommenda-

    tion in regard to examination of treatment packages,whereby we argued that evidence for the package does

    not imply evidence for the components and vice versa.

    Although not without drawbacks, we believe this

    approach has the greatest chance of encouraging

    researchers and clinicians to select measures and treat-

    ment components carefully, and to avoid adding measures

    or treatments in the absence of evidence for their incre-

    mental usefulness.

    Recommendation 4

    Finally, and at the risk of sounding prescriptive, we

    encourage researchers to be explicit and emphatic about

    what questions their studies are and are not equipped to

    answer. No study is perfect, and no study answers all

    questions; hence, we encourage open acknowledgement of

    design limitations that influence interpretation of study

    results. There are no penultimate studiesonly studies

    designed to answer specific questions. Researchers are

    encouraged to resist the temptation to go beyond their data

    in drawing conclusions and to correct others, including

    media representatives, who may portray their study results

    in an overly zealous fashion. Most often such misrepre-

    sentations are unintentional and a result of

    misunderstanding of the nuances of the study designsan

    understandable problem among lay consumers. We

    encourage researchers to correct these misunderstandings,

    even if doing so requires great effort and additional work.

    This is critical as our work is read not only by our col-

    leagues, but also guides policy issues, affects insurance

    coverage and directs best practices guidelines. Interpret

    each and every study cautiously, as if your own childs or

    Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334 331

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    15/17

    grandchilds future may be affected by it; in fact, it likely

    will be.

    Limitations

    Just as no research study answers all study questions, so

    too, no review covers all pertinent issues. Our focus was onseveral specific issues, at the expense of others that,

    admittedly, may be equally or even more important. Before

    closing, we wish to make some of these limitations explicit,

    in the hope that other researchers may tackle some of these

    issues that we leave unaddressed.

    A key unaddressed issue has to do with the effects of

    comorbidity on outcomes in incremental benefit studies.

    Given the overwhelming number of comparisons already

    summarized in Table1, we chose not to include comor-

    bidity comparisons in the subset of studies that reported

    them. However, as work by Jensen et al. (2001) has dem-

    onstrated, different comorbid subgroups of children withADHD may respond better or worse to different treatments.

    Hence, some of the lack of clarity of the big picture we

    describe herein, might be clarified by such an analysis of

    comorbid subgroups. We leave this task to a future team of

    investigators.

    Similarly, we did not evaluate the evidence separately

    by age, gender, length of treatment, or differences in

    sample characteristics (e.g., chosen by comprehensive

    clinical assessments vs. DSM-based rating scales).

    Although we include descriptive data in regard to some of

    these characteristics in Table1, an in-depth analysis is

    needed to clearly elucidate their effects. Again, this

    remains a task for future work.

    Finally, the issues we chose to highlight are admittedly

    controversial ones. Many viewpoints are possible, and

    considering differing perspectives is an important part of

    the scientific process. We are certain that we have not

    captured all possible issues, and even in regard to the issues

    we chose to address, additional viewpoints were left un-

    explicated. We hope that other researchers will assist in

    filling the gaps we leave unaddressed and the perspectives

    that we leave unexplicated. Nonetheless, if this article

    serves to stimulate discussion regarding the importance of

    design issues to study outcomes, and if it promotes con-

    structive discussion about how to improve future work,

    then we have accomplished our goals.

    Summary

    The goal of this article was to outline issues critical to

    evaluating the literature on incremental benefit of multiple

    effective treatments used together, vs. a single effective

    treatment, for childhood ADHD. These issues included: (1)

    sequencing and dosage of treatments being combined and

    compared; (2) difficulty drawing valid conclusions about

    individual components of treatment when treatment pack-

    ages are employed; (3) differing results emerging from

    measurement tools that purportedly measure the same

    domain; and (4) the resultant difficulty in reaching a

    summary conclusion when multiple outcome measuresyielding conflicting results are used. The implications of

    these issues for the design and conduct of future studies

    were discussed, with a particular emphasis on the need to

    develop and validate measurement models that can guide

    research.

    Acknowledgments During the preparation of this manuscript, all

    authors were supported in part by a grant from the National Institute

    of Mental Health to the first author: R01 MH065899. The first author

    also was supported in part by N01 MH12010 to B.S.G. Molina; and

    the first and third authors were supported in part by R43 MH076359

    to A. Terrazas. The views represented in this article are solely those of

    the authors.

    References

    Abikoff, H., Hechtman, L., Klein, R. G., Gallagher, R., Fleiss, K.,

    Etcovitch, J., et al. (2004a). Social functioning in children with

    ADHD treated with long-term methylphenidate and multimodal

    psychosocial treatment. Journal of the American Academy of

    Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 820829.

    Abikoff, H., Hechtman, L., Klein, R. G., Weiss, G., Fleiss, K.,

    Etcovitch, J., et al. (2004b). Symptomatic improvement in

    children with ADHD treated with long-term methylphenidate

    and multimodal psychosocial treatment.Journal of the American

    Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 802811.

    American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical

    manual of mental disorders(4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Arnold, L. E., Chuang, S., Davies, M., Abikoff, H. B., Conners, C. K.,

    Elliott, G. R., et al. (2004). Nine months of multicomponent

    behavioral treatment for ADHD and effectiveness of MTA

    fading procedures. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32,

    3951.

    Barkley, R. A. (2000). Commentary on the multimodal treatment

    study of children with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child

    Psychology, 28, 595599.

    Barkley, R. A., Shelton, T. L., Crosswait, C., Moorehouse, M.,

    Fletcher, K., Barrett, S., et al. (2000). Multi-method psycho-

    educational intervention for preschool children with disruptive

    behavior: Preliminary results at post-treatment. Journal of Child

    Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 319332.Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral

    sciences(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Cunningham, C. E. (1999). In the wake of the MTA: Charting a new

    course for the study and treatment of children with attention-

    deficit hyperactivity disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry,

    44, 9991006.

    Ercan, E. S., Varan, A., & Deniz, U. (2005). Effects of combined

    treatment on Turkish children diagnosed with attention-deficit/

    hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary report. Journal of Child

    and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15, 203219.

    Flory, K., Molina, B. S. G., Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E., & Smith, B.

    (2006). Childhood ADHD predicts risky sexual behavior in

    332 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    16/17

    young adulthood. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent

    Psychology, 35, 571577.

    Hechtman, L., Abikoff, H., Klein, R. G., Greenfield, B., Etcovitch, J.,

    Cousins, L., et al. (2004a). Children with ADHD treated with

    long-term methylphenidate and multimodal psychosocial treat-

    ment: Impact on parental practices. Journal of the American

    Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 830838.

    Hechtman, L., Abikoff, H., Klein, R. G., Weiss, G., Respitz, C.,

    Kouri, J., et al (2004b). Academic achievement and emotional

    status of children with ADHD treated with long-term methyl-

    phenidate and multimodal psychosocial treatment.Journal of the

    American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43,

    812819.

    Hoza, B., Gerdes, A. C., Mrug, S., Hinshaw, S. P., Bukowski, W. M.,

    Gold, J. A., et al. (2005). Peer-assessed outcomes in the

    multimodal treatment study of children with attention deficit

    hyperactivity disorder.Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent

    Psychology, 34, 7486.

    Hoza, B., Johnston, C., Pillow, D. R., & Ascough, J. C. (2006).

    Predicting treatment response for childhood attention-deficit/

    hyperactivity disorder: Introduction of a heuristic model to guide

    research.Applied and Preventive Psychology, 11, 215229.

    Hoza, B., Kaiser, N. M., & Hurt, E. (in press). Evidence-based

    treatments for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

    In M. Roberts, D. Elkin, & R. Steele (Eds.), Handbook of

    evidence based therapies for children and adolescents. New

    York: Springer.

    Hoza, B., Owens, J. S., & Pelham, W. E. (1999). Attention-deficit/

    hyperactivity disorder. In R.T. Ammerman, M. Hersen, & C. G.

    Last (Eds.), Handbook of prescriptive treatments for children

    and adolescents (2nd ed., pp. 6383). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Jensen, P. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Kraemer, H. C., Lenora N., Newcorn J.

    H., Abikoff, H. B., et al. (2001). ADHD comorbidity findings

    from the MTA study: Comparing comorbid subgroups. Journal

    of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40,

    147158.

    Klein, R. G., & Abikoff, H. (1997). Behavior therapy and methyl-

    phenidate in the treatment of children with ADHD. Journal of

    Attention Disorders, 2, 89114.

    Klein, R. G., Abikoff, H., Hechtman, L., & Weiss, G. (2004). Design

    and rationale of controlled study of long-term methylphenidate

    and multimodal psychosocial treatment in children with ADHD.

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

    Psychiatry, 43, 792801.

    Kolko, D. J., Bukstein, O. G., & Barron, J. (1999). Methylphenidate

    and behavior modification in children with ADHD and comorbid

    ODD or CD: Main and incremental effects across settings.

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

    Psychiatry, 38, 578586.

    Mash, E. J., & Hunsley, J. (2005). Evidence-based assessment of

    child and adolescent disorders: Issues and challenges.Journal of

    Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 34, 362379.

    Molina, B. S. G., Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E. M., Thompson, A. L., &

    Marshal, M. P. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorderrisk for heavy drinking and alcohol use disorder is age specific.

    Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 31, 643654.

    Mrug, S., Hoza, B., & Gerdes, A. C. (2001). Children with attention-

    deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Peer relationships and peer-ori-

    ented interventions. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), D. W. Nangle, &

    C. A. Erdley (Eds.), New directions for child and adolescent

    development: No. 91. The role of friendship in psychological

    adjustment(pp. 5177). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    MTA Cooperative Group (1999). A 14-month randomized clinical

    trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity

    disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 10731086.

    Owens, J. S., & Hoza, B. (2003). The role of inattention and

    hyperactivity/impulsivity in the positive illusory bias. Journal of

    Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 680691.

    Pelham, W. E. (1999). The NIMH multimodal treatment study for

    attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Just say yes to drugs

    alone? Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 981990.

    Pelham, W. E., Burrows-MacLean, L., Gnagy, E. M., Fabiano, G. A.,

    Coles, E. K., Tresco, K. E., et al. (2005a). Transdermal

    methylphenidate, behavioral, and combined treatment for chil-

    dren with ADHD. Experimental and Clinical

    Psychopharmacology, 13, 111126.

    Pelham, W. E., Erhardt, D., Gnagy, E. M., Greiner, A. R., Arnold, L.

    E., Abikoff, H. B., et al. (2007). Parent and teacher evaluation

    of treatment in the MTA: Consumer satisfaction and perceived

    effectiveness.University at Buffalo (submitted).

    Pelham, W. E., Fabiano, G. A., Gnagy, E. M., Greiner, A. R., & Hoza,

    B. (2005b). The role of summer treatment programs in the

    context of comprehensive treatment for attention-deficit/hyper-

    activity disorder. In E. D. Hibbs & P. S. Jensen (Eds.),

    Psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent disorders:

    Empirically based strategies for clinical practice (2nd ed., pp.

    377409). Washington, DC: American Psychological

    Association.

    Pelham, W. E., Fabiano, G. A., & Massetti, G. M. (2005c). Evidence-

    based assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in

    children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adoles-

    cent Psychology, 34, 449476.

    Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E. M., Greiner, A. R., Hoza, B., Hinshaw, S.

    P., Swanson, J. M., et al. (2000). Behavioral versus behavioral

    and pharmacological treatment in ADHD children attending a

    summer treatment program. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-

    chology, 28, 507525.

    Pelham, W. E., & Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Behavioral intervention for

    attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. In S. M. Turner, K. D.

    Calhoun, & H. E. Adams (Eds.), Handbook of clinical behavior

    therapy (Vol. 2, pp. 259283). New York: Wiley.

    Pelham, W. E., & Hoza, B. (1996). Intensive treatment: A summer

    treatment program for children with ADHD. In E. D. Hibbs & P.

    S. Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial treatments for child and adoles-

    cent disorders: Empirically based strategies for clinical practice

    (pp. 311340). Washington, DC: American Psychological

    Association.

    Pelham, W. E., Wheeler, T., & Chronis, A. (1998). Empirically

    supported psychosocial treatments for attention deficit hyperac-

    tivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 190

    205.

    Pfiffner, L. J., & McBurnett, K. (1997). Social skills training with

    parent generalization: Treatment effects for children with

    attention deficit disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

    Psychology, 65, 749757.

    Reitman, D., Hupp, S. D. A., OCallaghan, P. M., Gulley, V., &

    Northup, J. (2001). The influence of a token economy and

    methylphenidate on attentive and disruptive behavior during

    sports with ADHD-diagnosed children. Behavior Modification,25, 305323.

    Richters, J. E., Arnold, L. E., Jensen, P. S., Abikoff, H., Conners, C.

    K., Greenhill, L. L., et al. (1995). NIMH collaborative multisite

    multimodal treatment study of children with ADHD: I. Back-

    ground and rationale. Journal of the American Academy of Child

    & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 9871000.

    Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for

    null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638641.

    Swanson, J. M., Elliott, G. R., Greenhill, L. L., Wigal, T., Arnold, L.

    E., Vitiello, B., et al. (2007). Effects of stimulant medication on

    growth rates across 3 years in the MTA follow-up. Journal of the

    Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334 333

    1 3

  • 8/10/2019 accesare_resursa.pdf

    17/17

    American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46,

    10151027.

    Thompson, A. L., Molina, B. S. G., Pelham, W., & Gnagy, E. M.

    (2007). Risky driving in adolescents and young adults with

    childhood ADHD.Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 745759.

    Tutty, S., Gephart, H., & Wurzbacher, K. (2003). Enhancing

    behavioral and social skill functioning in children newly diag-

    nosed with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in a pediatric

    setting.Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 24, 5157.

    van der Oord, S., Prins, P. J. M., Oosterlaan, J., & Emmelkamp, P. M.

    G. (2007). Does brief, clinically based, intensive multimodal

    behavior therapy enhance the effects of methylphenidate in

    children with ADHD? European Child and Adolescent Psychi-

    atry, 16, 4857.

    Volkow, N. D., & Insel, T. R. (2003). What are the long-term effects

    of methylphenidate treatment? Biological Psychiatry, 54,

    13071309.

    Wells, K. C., Chi, T. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Epstein, J. N., Pfiffner, L.,

    Nebel-Schwalm, M., et al. (2006). Treatment-related changes in

    objectively measured parenting behaviors in the multimodal

    treatment study of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

    disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74,

    649657.

    Wells, K. C., Epstein, J. N., Hinshaw, S. P., Conners, C. K., Klaric, J.,

    Abikoff, H. B., et al. (2000). Parenting and family stress

    treatment outcomes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

    (ADHD): An empirical analysis in the MTA study. Journal of

    Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 543553.

    334 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10:318334

    1 3