05cercetare

download 05cercetare

of 14

Transcript of 05cercetare

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    1/14

    2000 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

    Peter M undyDepartment of Psychology

    University of MiamiMiami, FL 33146

    Judith Card

    N athan FoxDepartment of Human Development

    University of MarylandCollege Park, MD 20740

    EEG Cor relates of the

    D evelopm ent of I nfant Joint

    Attention Skills

    Received 19 August 1999; accepted 7 December 1999

    ABSTRACT: The development of the capacity for social attention coordination, or jointattention, is a major milestone of infancy. Data from a recent study of handicapped infantshave raised the hypothesis that the tendency to initiate bids for joint attention may reflect

    processes associated with the frontal cortex to a greater extent than other forms of infantattention coordination (R. Caplan et al., 1993). This hypothesis was examined in a longitudinalstudy of 32 normally developing infants. The results indicated that EEG data at 14 monthsindicative of left frontal, as well as left and right central cortical activity, was associated withthe tendency to initiate joint attention bids (IJA) at 14 and 18 months. In contrast, a patternof left parietal activation and right parietal deactivation at 14 months was associated with thedevelopment of the capacity to respond to the joint attention bids (RJA) of others at 14 and 18months. These results were interpreted to be consistent with a general anterior posterior modelof attention development (M. Posner & S. Petersen, 1990). The implications of these results

    for current conceptualizations of joint attention development, as well as for understanding thedisturbance of joint attention skill development in autism are discussed. 2000 John Wiley &Sons Inc. Dev Psychobiol 36: 325338, 2000

    Keywords: EEG; joint attention; infant development

    The acquisition of the ability to coordinate attention

    with a social partner is a major milestone of infancy

    that is critical to infants active participation in social

    learning opportunities (e.g., Adamson, 1995; Baldwin,

    1993; Butterworth & Jarrett, 1991; Corkum & Moore,

    1997; Mundy & Willoughby, 1998; Scaife & Bruner,

    1975; Trevarthen, 1979; Tomasello, Kruger, & Ratner,

    1993; Werner & Kaplan, 1963). Consistent with this

    view, individual differences in this capacity among in-

    fants have been observed to be significantly related to

    Correspondence to: P. MundyContract grant sponsor: NICHDContract grant number: 26768Contract grant sponsor: NIDCDContract grant number: 00484

    intellectual, language, and social development (e.g.,

    Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998; Claussen,

    Mundy, & Willoughby, 1999; Morales, Mundy, & Ro-

    jas, 1998; Mundy & Gomes, 1998; Sheinkopf, Mundy,

    Willoughby, & Claussen, 1999; Tomasello; 1995; Ul-

    vund & Smith, 1996). Thus, understanding the nature

    of individual differences in infant attention coordina-

    tion is an important issue in the study of early devel-

    opment (Mundy & Gomes, 1998).

    Understanding these individual differences, how-

    ever, is complicated by the observation that there are

    different types of infant attention coordinating behav-

    iors that may reflect distinct as well as common pro-

    cesses (Mundy & Gomes, 1997). One category of be-

    haviors has been referred to as protoimperative, or

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    2/14

    326 Mundy, Card, and Fox

    requesting skills, in which infants initiate or respond

    to bids for coordinated attention to elicit aid in obtain-

    ing an object or event (Bates, Benigni, Bretherton, &

    Camaioni, 1979; Bruner & Sherwood, 1983; Seibert,

    Hogan, & Mundy, 1982). Examples include using eye

    contact and reaching to obtain a distal toy, or respond-ing to a palm-up hand gesture to give an object to a

    social partner. Another category of coordinated atten-

    tion involves a less instrumental and more social func-

    tion of sharing experience with others vis-a-vis an ob-

    ject or event. These are called joint attention or

    protodeclarative behaviors (Bates et al., 1979; Bruner

    & Sherwood, 1983). As with requesting skill, infants

    may initiate or respond to joint attention bids. In the

    nomenclature of this article, responding to joint atten-

    tion (RJA; Seibert et al., 1982) refers to the ability to

    monitor and correctly follow gestures, such as point-

    ing, and/or a shift in the direction of gaze and the head

    orientation of others (Butterworth & Jarrett, 1991;Scaife & Bruner, 1975). Initiating joint attention skill

    (IJA; Seibert et al., 1982) refers to the use of eye con-

    tact and gestures, such as showing or alternating eye

    contact, to spontaneously establish episodes of shared

    attention with others vis-a-vis some object or event

    (Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1975; Mundy, Kasari,

    & Sigman, 1992).

    Recently, data have been presented that suggest that

    individual differences in IJA may display a specific

    association with frontal cortical processes. Caplan et

    al. (1993) used Positron Emission Tomography (PET)

    to examine cortical metabolism in a small sample of13 infants and toddlers prior to hemispherectomy for

    intractable seizure disorder. Higher rates of preopera-

    tive glucose metabolism in the prefrontal region, es-

    pecially the left frontal region, was a positive predictor

    of the postoperative tendency to initiate joint attention

    bids but was not a predictor of other types of social

    attention coordination skills. Thus, processes associ-

    ated with the left frontal cortex may distinguish the

    development of the tendency to initiate joint attention

    bids relative to other infant attention coordinating be-

    haviors. Several lines of theory are consistent with this

    observation.

    Working memory and related dual-task processing

    (Stuss, Shallice, Alexander, & Picton, 1995), as well

    as the representational encoding of stimuli (Goldman-

    Rakic, 1987), may be a fundamental function of the

    frontal cortex. Initiating joint attention skill may also

    involve a working memory and representational de-

    mands wherein infants hold information on line about

    their own focus of interest (e.g., a toy) while attending

    to and interacting with a social partner. Moreover,

    these demands may be greater in IJA than in other

    types of social attention coordinating bids, such as re-

    questing acts (Baron-Cohen, 1989, 1995).

    Frontal functioning may also be involved in the de-

    velopment of infants ability to inhibit responses. For

    example, infants are increasingly able to tolerate delay

    in the A not B task in the first year of life (Diamond,1988) and this has been related to increased left frontal

    EEG activation (Bell & Fox, 1992). Initiating joint

    attention may involve inhibiting visual regard for an

    object in order to share attention to a greater extent

    than requesting skills (McEvoy, Rogers, & Penning-

    ton, 1993). Indeed, behavioral response inhibition has

    been associated with initiating joint attention skill, but

    not other forms of infant attention coordination be-

    haviors, in developmentally disordered children

    (McEvoy et al., 1993; Giffith, Pennington, Wehner, &

    Rogers, 1999).

    Finally, in addition to cognitive processes, the de-

    gree to which spontaneously sharing attention be-comes rewarding for infants may effect their tendency

    to initiate joint attention bids (Mundy, 1995; Mundy

    et al., 1992; Mundy & Willoughby, 1998). Alterna-

    tively, social reward sensitivity or experience may

    play a lesser role in the more instrumental functions

    of requesting bids (Mundy, 1995). Frontal systems, in

    turn, may play a role in integrating the reinforcement

    value of the stimuli or motivational processes with

    cognitive factors in the regulation of behavior

    (Thorpe, Rolls, & Maddison, 1983). Moreover, a func-

    tional lateralization has been observed such that, by

    10 months, behaviors associated with positive indicesof social motivation, including social orienting, posi-

    tive affect, and approach behaviors, are also related to

    left frontal processes (Fox, 1991; Fox & Davidson,

    1987). Thus, individual differences in a left frontal

    system involved in the mediation of positive social

    motivation may play a more important role in the de-

    velopment of the capacity to initiate joint attention

    bids than in other forms of infant attention coordina-

    tion (Mundy, 1995; Mundy & Willoughby, 1998).

    These elements of theory make a cogent case for

    the involvement of frontal processes in IJA develop-

    ment. However, the results of Caplan et al. (1993),

    which provide an initial basis for this hypothesis, may

    only be viewed as preliminary because they were

    based on a small sample whose development was com-

    plicated by severe neurological disturbance. If bona

    fide, though, the observation of a specific linkage be-

    tween frontal functions and initiating joint attention

    skill development would be important for several rea-

    sons. Such an observation may be important to under-

    standing the nature of the robust disturbance in initi-

    ating joint attention skill development that is a cardinal

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    3/14

    Cortical Activity and Joint Attention 327

    symptom of autism (Mundy, 1995). Furthermore, it

    may also enrich the understanding of the observed

    connections between individual differences in infant

    IJA skill and childhood IQ (Ulvund & Smith, 1996),

    language development (Mundy & Gomes, 1998), at-

    tachment (Claussen et al., 1999), and childhood socialcompetence (Sheinkopf et al., 1999; Sigman & Rus-

    kin, 1999). Therefore, this study was designed to pro-

    vide an a priori test of the frontal mediation hypothesis

    of initiating joint attention in a sample of infants who

    were not effected by neurological disturbance.

    The question arises, however, as to how best to

    investigate this putative brainbehavior relation.

    Caplan et al. (1993) observed neurological predic-

    tors of IJA development using baseline rather than

    event-related measures of brain activity. Therefore, to

    maintain consistency with the methodology of Caplan

    et al. (1993), baseline measure of brain activity were

    used in this study. However, here baseline EEG ratherthan PET data were utilized.

    In this regard, it has been argued that baseline EEG

    data may be a valid marker of important aspects of

    brain maturation and integration that precedes the

    emergence of skills at different points in infancy (Fi-

    scher & Rose, 1994; Hudspeth & Pribram, 1992;

    Thatcher, 1994), especially visual attention skills (Par-

    melee et al., 1994). Thatcher (1994) has also argued

    for the use of measures of baseline EEG coherence to

    examine aspects of brain and behavior maturation. Co-

    herence is a measure of phase synchrony or shared

    activity between spatially separated [EEG] generators(Thatcher, 1994, p. 233). A two-component model of

    coherence has been proposed in which maturation of

    the left hemisphere may be reflected in a pattern of

    integration of differentiation or a reduction in co-

    herence among proximal cortical sites, followed by an

    increase in coherence among more distant cortical sites

    in baseline EEG (pp. 251254). Maturation of right

    hemisphere functions may be characterized by dif-

    ferentiation of integration (pp. 251254) or a reduc-

    tion of distal coherence followed by an increase in

    proximal coherence among cortical areas. Further-

    more, coherence measures are assumed to reflect the

    integration or organization of brain processes that are

    involved in the acquisition phase of a skill. Thus, co-

    herence measures may be markers of differences in

    brain integration processes that precede the acquisition

    or consolidation of a skill (Bell & Fox, 1996; Fischer

    & Rose, 1994).

    In accordance with this literature, baseline EEG

    measures of brain activity and cortical coherence were

    used in this study to examine the frontal mediation

    hypothesis of initiating joint attention skill develop-

    ment in the 14- to 18-month period, a period of con-

    solidation for this skill (Adamson, 1995).

    M ETH O D S

    Participants

    Thirty-six healthy, full-term 14-month-olds (18 girls,

    18 boys) participated in this study. All of these tod-

    dlers were born within 2 weeks of their due dates.

    They required no oxygen or major medical interven-

    tion after birth and were negative for neurological dis-

    orders or complications. The toddlers were recruited

    from a major metropolitan area via mailing lists com-

    posed of parents with middle to upper-middle SES and

    a minimum education of graduation from high school.

    Of the mothers of children in this study, 3 (9%) had

    advanced degrees or training, 25 (78%) had completedsome or all of college, and 4 (13%) had completed

    high school. Ethnicity of the mothers was primarily

    Caucasian American (29, 91%), two were Black

    American and one was Latin American. Only toddlers

    born to two-right handed parents were recruited for

    this study. Missing or compromised EEG data on 4

    infants reduced the sample to 32 infants (14 girls, 18

    boys) with complete longitudinal data on all measures.

    Procedures

    The electrophysiological and joint attention data col-lection required approximately 90 min, and the tod-

    dlers were seen within 10 days of each of their 14-,

    16-, and 18-month birthdays.

    Electrophysiological Recording. Brain electrical ac-

    tivity (EEG) was recorded during the 14-month visit

    from 12 head sites: frontal, central, temporal, parietal,

    and occipital regions for both the left and right hem-

    ispheres (F3, F4, F7, F8, C3, C4, T3, T4, P3, P4, O1,

    O2) referenced to the vertex (Cz). Facial activity, such

    as mouth and jaw movements, crying, or changes in

    facial affect, may be expected to affect data from F7,

    F8, T3, and T4 to a greater extent than from other sites.

    Visual inspection from these sites confirmed that in a

    number of instances these data were confounded with

    high-frequency motor artifact. Therefore, data from

    these sites were eliminated from the analyses.

    Baseline EEG was recorded for 3 min while the

    toddler sat quietly in a highchair next to his or her

    mother. During EEG recording, the toddler attended

    to a novel display of a rotating bingo wheel with

    brightly colored balls. This procedure has been found

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    4/14

    328 Mundy, Card, and Fox

    to be successful in garnering extended bouts of visual

    attention while keeping the toddler quiet with reduced

    eye movement and motor activity during the 3-min

    period. The mother was instructed to refrain from talk-

    ing or moving during the recording. The EEG was

    recorded using a stretch lycra cap containing elec-trodes in the 10/20 placement system pattern (Jasper,

    1958). After measuring the toddlers head to determine

    the appropriate cap size and placing the cap on the

    toddler, small amounts of abrasive gel and conductive

    gel were inserted into the electrodes of interest. The

    blunt end of a q-tip was used to gently abrade the scalp

    after inserting each type of gel. Electrode impedances

    were measured and accepted if below 5K ohms. Ad-

    ditional abrading was sometimes necessary if initial

    impedances measured greater than 5K ohms. Eye

    movement (EOG) was recorded using Beckman min-

    iature electrodes placed on the external canthus and

    the supra orbit of the right eye.Prior to the recording of each subject, a 10-Hz .477

    Vrms sine wave was input through each amplifier of

    the polygraph. The amplifiers were set so that output

    of the signal represented 50 uV peak to peak, with a

    gain of 10,000. The EEG from each site of interest

    was amplified using separate Grass amplifiers (Model

    7P11) and bandpassed from 1 to 100 Hz, with a notch

    filter at 60 Hz. The EEG signal was digitized on-line

    at 512 samples per second for each channel to ensure

    the data were not affected by aliasing. The raw data

    were stored for later analysis. The EEG data were an-

    alyzed using software by James Long Company. First,the EEG data were re-referenced via software to an

    average reference configuration. The average refer-

    enced EEG data were scored for eye movement and

    motor artifact, and epochs containing artifacts were

    eliminated from analysis. The data were analyzed with

    a Discrete Fourier Transform (DFT) using a Hanning

    window of 1 s with a 50% overlap. Prior to the com-

    putation of the DFT, the mean voltage was subtracted

    from each data point to eliminate any power results

    due to DC offset. Power was computed for the 4- to

    6-Hz and the 6- to 9-Hz frequency band. Past research

    with infants has demonstrated that the majority of

    spectral power can be found in this range, and espe-

    cially these two bandwidths (see Calkins, Fox, & Mar-

    shall, 1996). The power was expressed as mean square

    microvolts and the data were transformed using the

    natural log (ln) to normalize the distribution.

    EEGANAL software was used to perform spectral

    coherence calculations across EEG sites by averaging

    the normalized complex cross-spectral density with

    bands and epochs after the method of Saltzberg,

    Burton, Burch, Fletcher, and Michaels (1986). Two

    types of coherence variables were considered in this

    study. A short-distance coherence variable between

    frontal and central sites was computed within the 4-

    to 6-Hz and 6- to 9-Hz bands for the 14-month EEG

    data, as was a long-distance coherence variable be-

    tween frontal and occipital sites. These two coherence

    variables were computed separately for left and righthemisphere sites. Similar to previously used methods

    (Thatcher, Krause, & Hrybyk, 1986), a summary co-

    herence score for each hemisphere was calculated re-

    flecting the ratio of short-distance coherence divided

    by long-distance coherence (FrontalCentral Coher-

    ence/FrontalOccipital Coherence).

    Joint Attention Measures. Data on nonverbal joint at-

    tention and requesting behaviors were collected with

    the revised Early Social Communication Scales

    (ESCS; Mundy, Hogan, & Doehring, 1996; Seibert et

    al., 1982). The ESCS is a 20-min videotaped struc-

    tured assessment designed to measure the develop-ment of a variety of nonverbal communication skills

    in the 6- to 30-month period. For this assessment an

    experimenter and an infant, sitting in his caregivers

    lap or independently in a child chair, are seated facing

    each other across a small table. A set of toys is visible

    to the child but out of reach on the experimenters side

    of the table. Posters are placed on the walls 90 degrees

    to the childs left and right, and 180 degrees behind

    the child. A video camera is positioned approximately

    10 feet behind the experimenter. The camera is ori-

    ented to capture a three-fourths face image of the child

    with a profile view of the experimenter as well as theposition of the toys and posters.

    The experimenter presents the child with a se-

    quence of three activated wind-up toys (three trials

    each), three hand-operated mechanical toys (three tri-

    als each), opportunities to play a tickle, turn-taking

    game (two trials), opportunities to play an object turn-

    taking game, such as catch with a ball (two trials),

    opportunities to take turns wearing a hat, comb, and

    glasses (three trials), and an opportunity to look at pic-

    tures in a book with the tester (one trial). The tester

    also presents the child with multiple requests to give

    toys to the tester. Finally, the tester presents the child

    with two sets of three trials in which the tester attracts

    the childs attention, and then turns to visually fixate

    a wall poster while pointing at the poster and saying

    the childs name three times with increasing emphasis.

    Trials to the left, right, and behind the child are con-

    ducted in each set.

    Observations of the testerchild interaction in the

    ESCS yielded frequency of behavior scores in six cat-

    egories: Initiating Joint Attention (IJA); Responding

    to Joint Attention (RJA), Initiating Behavioral Re-

    questing (IBR), Responding to Behavioral Requesting

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    5/14

    Cortical Activity and Joint Attention 329

    Table 1. Brief Description of the Early Social

    Communication Scale Variables

    INITIATES JOINT ATTENTION

    (a) Makes eye contact while manipulating toy; (b) alter-

    nates eye contact between active mechanical toy and tes-

    ter; (c) points to active mechanical toy or distal objects intesting room; (d) shows objects (raises objects toward the

    testers face).

    RESPONDS TO JOINT ATTENTION

    The percentage of trials the child correctly turns head and

    eyes in direction of testers point. On side trials, direction

    of gaze must shift beyond the testers extended finger (ap-

    proximately 45 degrees off midline). On behind trials, the

    child must turn head more than 90 degrees off midline.

    INITIATES BEHAVIOR REQUESTS

    (a) Makes eye contact when object moved out of reach or

    reaches to objects out of reach; (b) Makes eye contact

    while reaching to objects out of reach; (c) Points to inac-

    tive objects on the table or to the collection of visible but

    out-of-reach toys; (d) Gives inactive toys to the tester

    (e.g., moves toys toward testers hands).

    RESPONDS TO BEHAVIOR REQUESTS

    The frequency of trials on which the child correctly re-

    sponds to testers gestural (i.e., palm-up hand extended)

    and verbal request to give it to me.

    (RBR), Initiating Social Interaction (ISI), and Re-

    sponding to Social Interaction (RSI). Both the Joint

    Attention and Behavioral Requesting involve the co-

    ordination of attention to objects and events. Alter-

    natively, the Social Interaction scales assess turn-tak-

    ing and interaction maintenance but not necessarilycoordination of attention to objects and events. There-

    fore, given the focus of this study, only data from the

    former will be considered in analyses. Briefly, these

    are operationalized in the following manner. IJA

    scores refer to the frequency with which the child

    spontaneously points, shows, and uses eye contact to

    share the experience of an active mechanical toy with

    the tester. In the age range in this study, the sponta-

    neous use of eye contact to alternate attention between

    an object and social partner is the most common be-

    havior scored on this measure. RJA refers to the per-

    centage of trials on which a child correctly turns her

    visual regard in the direction of the testers visual re-gard and pointing gesture. IBR scores refer to the fre-

    quency with which the child uses eye contact, reach-

    ing, giving, and pointing to elicit aid in obtaining

    objects or reactivating objects. RBR refers to the fre-

    quency of trials on which a child correctly responds

    to the experimenters hand out, palm up gesture and

    verbalization of give it to me while the child is hold-

    ing or examining a toy. A more complete description

    of the behaviors observed within each category is pre-

    sented in Table 1. The raters were blind to the other

    data collection procedures as well as the hypotheses

    of this study.Reliability for the ESCS measures used in this

    study are well established (e.g., Mundy & Gomes,

    1998; Mundy, Kasari, Sigman, & Ruskin, 1995;

    Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1994). Estimates of inter-

    rater reliability based on 12 independent paired obser-

    vations of data for this study yielded rs .87, .95,

    .82, & .79, p .001, for the total frequency scores for

    IJA, RJA, IBR, and RBR, respectively. IJA displayed

    a significant degree of stability (test-retest reliability)

    in this study with correlations among the 14-, 16-, and

    18-month IJA measures ranging from r(31) .57 to

    .83, p .001. RJA also displayed evidence of stability

    between the 14-month and 18-month measure,

    r(31) .51, p .002. The relations between the 14-

    and 16-month RJA measure and 16- and 18-month

    measure approached significance, r(31) .25 and .28,

    p .08 and .06, respectively. The IBR and RBR mea-

    sures displayed no evidence of significant stability in

    this study, r(31) range .13 to .24. Thus, the IJA

    and RJA measures displayed more optimal psycho-

    metric characteristics than did IBR and RBR in this

    study.

    Covariance among the different types of infant at-

    tention coordination measures was modest. Of the pos-

    sible 54 interscale correlations among the 14-, 16-, and18-month ESCS measures, just 12 were significant.

    Across the types of joint attention measures, only 14-

    month RJA was correlated with 14-month IJA, r

    .36, p .05 (one tailed), and 16-month IJA, r .33,

    p .05. However, at 18 months the IJA and RJA

    correlation was r .05. IJA and IBR were not signif-

    icantly correlated, but IJA was correlated with RBR at

    18 months, r .34, p .05. Thus, the different ESCS

    measures appeared to assess unique or distinct aspects

    of the development of social attention coordination

    skill in this study.

    RESU LTS

    D evelopment of Joint Attention Skil ls

    The means of the four types of infant attention coor-

    dination skills at the three ages of assessment are pre-

    sented in Table 2. Preliminary analyses were con-

    ducted to evaluate the effect of gender and age-related

    changes on these measures. Because of their difference

    in scaling (percentage data), RJA scores were first con-

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    6/14

    330 Mundy, Card, and Fox

    Table 2. Means, with Standard Deviations in Parentheses, for the ESCS Variables at 14, 16, and 18 Months of Age

    ESCS Variables 14 Months 16 Months 18 Months

    Initiates joint attention (IJA)

    Girls 19.5 (10.4) 16.1 (9.5) 19.8 (8.8)

    Boys 15.9 (8.6) 14.7 (10.2) 16.3 (8.2)

    Total 16.5 (8.0) 15.4 (10.1) 17.5 (8.6)Responds to joint attention (RJA)

    Girls 87% (12%) 78% (25%) 81% (27%)

    Boys 76% (25%) 81% (20%) 83% (23%)

    Total 82% (20%) 81% (21%) 83% (24%)

    Initiates behavior regulation (IBR)

    Girls 33.6 (11.3) 35.0 (10.2) 41.5 (7.3)

    Boys 30.5 (13.2) 33.6 (10.6) 36.1 (14.4)

    Total 32.4 (12.0) 34.0 (10.7) 38.2 (12.4)

    Responds to behavior regulation (RBR)

    Girls 2.9 (1.2) 3.2 (1.3) 3.5 (1.4)

    Boys 3.5 (1.5) 3.2 (1.7) 4.1 (2.3)

    Total 3.4 (1.4) 3.2 (1.5) 3.9 (2.1)

    sidered in a separate ANOVA using arc sine transfor-

    mations. This analysis revealed no effects of gender

    or age for RJA in this study. Next, a 2 3 3 (Gen-

    der Age ESCS Scores) ANOVA was computed

    for the IJA, IBR, and RBR variables. No effects as-

    sociated with gender were significant. The main effect

    for age was significant, F(2, 62) 3.68, p .05, but

    the Age ESCS Scores interaction was not signifi-

    cant.

    The main effect for age indicated that the children

    displayed a general increase in facility with coordi-nated attention bids with age (see Table 2). Pairwise

    bonferronni comparisons for the main effect for age

    revealed that 18-month-olds displayed significantly

    higher ESCS scores than did 14-or 16-month-olds,

    p .05. The 14- and 16-month-olds did not differ,

    p .74. Within individual scales of the ESCS, this

    effect reached significance for IBR on the 18-versus

    14-month comparison, p .05. Therefore, to reduce

    the data and constrain the number of analyses, only

    data from the 14- and 18-month measures were con-

    sidered in examinations of the relations between EEG

    and ESCS variables in this study.

    EEG Prediction of Joint Attention and O therN onverb al Skil ls

    The covariance among the EEG data from the eight

    leads (two frontal, two central, two parietal, and two

    occipital) was considerable. For example, the average

    correlation among the eight leads for the 4- to 6-Hz

    band data was: r(30) .65, range .30 to .87. Av-

    erage covariance for 6- to 9-Hz data was even

    stronger: r .72, range .42 to .92. Consequently,

    hierarchical multiple regression analyses (Cohen &

    Cohen, 1983) were conducted to determine if 14-

    month baseline EEG indices of left frontal activity

    would display significant and unique predictive rela-

    tions with the development of individual differences

    in 18-month IJA. Analyses were also computed to

    compare the associations of 14-month EEG data with

    the three other ESCS measures at 18 monthsRJA,

    IBR, and RBR. In each analysis, data from the left and

    right hemisphere ratio EEG coherence scores (frontal

    central coherence/frontal/occipital coherence) wereentered on Step 1. On Steps 2, 3, 4, and 5, the log

    power data from the eight EEG leads (left and right

    frontal, central, parietal, and occipital pairs) were en-

    tered, respectively. This set order of data entry re-

    flected the a priori emphasis in examining the relations

    of ratio coherence and frontal EEG data with individ-

    ual differences in joint attention. In addition, the 14-

    month IJA and RJA scores were significantly corre-

    lated with their respective 18-month scores (see

    Methods) and were entered on the first step of analyses

    of the 18-month IJA and RJA scores, respectively.

    These regression analyses yielded a set of significant

    equations for several of the different types of infant

    attention coordination skills using EEG data from the

    4- to 6-Hz band. However, none of the regression

    equations using the 6- to 9-Hz band EEG data were

    significant. Therefore, only the results of the former

    have been reported in this article.

    The results of the analyses for IJA and RJA are

    presented in Tables 3 and 4. To be considered signif-

    icant, a step in these analyses needed to be associated

    both with a significant increase in R2 as well as a sig-

    nificant overall effect for the regression equation. Data

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    7/14

    Cortical Activity and Joint Attention 331

    Table 3. Significant 14-Month EEG Predictors of the 18-Month Initiating Joint Attention

    Steps and Variables R R2a p

    Step 1 (df 3/28) .67 .39 .001*

    Left hemisphere coherence .37 .02*

    Right hemisphere coherence .03

    14-month IJA .49 .002*Step 2 (df 5/26) .69 .38 .003*

    Left hemisphere coherence .35 .04*

    Right hemisphere coherence .06

    14-month IJA .53 .002*

    Left frontal EEG .28

    Right frontal EEG .30

    Step 3 (df 7/24)b .77 .48 .001* (.05*)

    Left hemisphere coherence .30 .05*

    Right hemisphere coherence .09

    14-month IJA .61 .001*

    Left frontal EEG .80 .025*

    Right frontal EEG .18

    Left central EEG .02

    Right central EEG .70 .05*

    Follow-up (df 4/27) .77 .52 .007*

    Left hemisphere coherence .28 .05*

    14-month IJA .61 .001*

    Left frontal EEG .68 .012*

    Left central EEG .73 .007*

    aR2 estimate adjusted for sample size. bThe p value for a significant change in R2 is presented in parentheses after the p value for the entire

    equation.

    are presented up through those steps that met these

    criteria. Follow-up analyses are also presented for

    equations obtained after the removal of blocks of var-iables with nonsignificant, p .10, standardized re-

    gression coefficients.

    Step 1 of the analyses of 18-month IJA was signif-

    icant (see Table 3). The 14-month IJA score was a

    significant positive predictor of 18-month IJA. In ad-

    dition, the left hemisphere ratio coherence score dis-

    played a significant negative . On Step 2 of these

    analyses, the addition of the left and right frontal EEG

    power data did not yield a significant increase in R2.

    However, on Step 3 a significant increase in R2 was

    observed in the analyses of 18-month IJA with the

    addition of 14-month central EEG power data (see Ta-

    ble 3). A positive (hypoactivation) right central EEG

    component at 14 months was a significant predictor of

    18-month IJA scores. Moreover, a suppressor effect

    was evident so that a unique and significant relation

    of left frontal activation at 14 months with 18-month

    IJA became apparent on this step after considering var-

    iance in coherence and central EEG data. Thus, Step

    3 of these analyses, as well as the follow-up analyses

    (see Table 3), revealed that a combination of 14-month

    baseline EEG measures including the left hemisphere

    ratio coherence measure, left frontal activation, and

    right central hypoactivation displayed unique paths of

    association with subsequent individual differences in

    IJA.To better understand the nature of the correlation

    between 18-month IJA and 14-month EEG coherence,

    we examined the individual components of this ratio

    measure. Coherence between left frontal and occipital

    sites at 14 months was not correlated with 18-month

    IJA, r .11, but coherence between left frontal and

    central sites was negatively correlated with 18-month

    IJA, r .48, p .005. This negative correlation

    suggests that lower coherence between left frontal and

    central cortical sites at 14 months, possibly indicative

    of individual differences in differentiation of left cor-

    tical activity (Thatcher, 1994), was related to IJA de-

    velopment at 18 months in this sample.

    The data in Table 4 revealed that the pattern of

    significant EEG predictors of RJA appeared to be quite

    different from the pattern of predictors of IJA. Aside

    from significant effects associated with 14-month RJA

    observed on Step 1, these analyses revealed a signifi-

    cant effect at Step 4 with the addition of the log power

    EEG data from the parietal electrodes. As can be seen

    most clearly in the follow-up analysis, after removing

    other EEG variables in the EEG data set, both indices

    of left parietal activation and right parietal hypoacti-

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    8/14

    332 Mundy, Card, and Fox

    Table 4. Significant 14-Month EEG Predictors of the 18-Month Responding to Joint Attention

    Steps and Variables R R2a p

    Step 1 (df 3/28) .56 .24 .012*

    Left hemisphere coherence .19

    Right hemisphere coherence .20

    14-month RJA .56 .002*Step 2 (df 5/26) .58 .21 .05*

    Left hemisphere coherence .18

    Right hemisphere coherence .15

    14-month RJA .55 .003*

    Left frontal EEG .08

    Right frontal EEG .19

    Step 3 (df 7/24)b .62 .21 .075

    Left hemisphere coherence .17

    Right hemisphere coherence .13

    14-month RJA .62 .001*

    Left frontal EEG .48

    Right frontal EEG .19

    Left central EEG .44

    Right central EEG .03

    Step 4 (df 9/22) .72 .33 .05* (.05*)

    Left hemisphere coherence .07

    Right hemisphere coherence .08

    14-month RJA .41 .05*

    Left frontal EEG .19

    Right frontal EEG .06

    Left central EEG .33

    Right central EEG .10

    Left parietal 1.10 .05*

    Right parietal .51

    Follow-up (df 3/28) .63 .33 .003*

    14-month RJA .40 .025*

    Left parietal .89 .025*Right parietal .85 .03*

    aR2 estimate adjusted for sample size. bThe p value for a significant change in R2 is presented in parentheses after the p value for the entire

    equation.

    vation displayed significant paths of association with

    RJA at 18 months (see Table 4).

    The analyses of IBR and RBR also revealed distinct

    patterns of data. None of the steps in the analyses of

    IBR approached significance. In these analyses, only

    activation of the left parietal EEG power data at 14

    months approached significance in predicting 18-

    month scores, .87, p .10. In contrast, a sig-

    nificant regression equation, R .57, F(4, 28) 3.32,

    p .025, and a significant increase in adjusted R2,

    R2 .16, F 4.01, p .03, was observed for the

    18-month RBR variable, on Step 2, with the entry of

    the frontal EEG log power data. However, unlike with

    the IJA data, a right frontal activation component,

    .82, p .009, and a left frontal hypoactivation,

    .72, p .02, were associated with IBR. Follow-

    up analyses revealed that these two frontal variables

    alone were associated with RBR, R .49, adjusted

    R2 .19, p .02; right frontal activation, .89,

    p .004, and left frontal hypoactivation, .71,

    p .02.

    Concurrent Correlat ions Betw een the EEG

    and ESCS VariablesThe patterns of concurrent multiple correlations be-

    tween the EEG and ESCS variables were also exam-

    ined at 14 and 18 months using hierarchical regression

    analyses with the same order of data entry as previ-

    ously described for the predictive regression equa-

    tions. The analyses of concurrent correlates of IJA at

    14 months yielded a marginally significant regression

    equation (see Table 5). Nevertheless, a combination of

    the left hemisphere frontal coherence ratio, as well as

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    9/14

    Cortical Activity and Joint Attention 333

    Table 5. Concurrent EEG Correlates of IJA at 14 Months Initiating Joint Attention

    Steps and Variables R R2a p

    Step 1 (df 2/29) .25 .01

    Left hemisphere coherence .26

    Right hemisphere coherence .11

    Step 2 (df 4/27) .38 .03Left hemisphere coherence .29

    Right hemisphere coherence .10

    Left frontal EEG .52

    Right frontal EEG .42

    Step 3 (df 6/25)b .53 .13 .078* (.078*)

    Left hemisphere coherence .35 .05*

    Right hemisphere coherence .11

    Left frontal EEG .42

    Right frontal EEG .20

    Left central EEG .68 .07*

    Right central EEG .78 .04*

    Follow-up (df 3/28) .49 .16 .007*

    Left hemisphere coherence .30 .06*

    Left central EEG .85 .009*

    Right central EEG .76 .02*

    aR2 estimate adjusted for sample size. bThe p value for a significant change in R2 is presented in parentheses after the p value for the entire

    equation.

    left central EEG hypoactivation and right central EEG

    activation, displayed unique paths of association with

    14-month IJA (see Table 5).

    In these data, though, neither of the individual com-

    ponents of the coherence measure were correlatedwith

    14-month IJA: left frontal and left central component,r .14, left frontal and left occipital component,

    r .13.

    The results of the other 14-month concurrent anal-

    yses may be summarized as follows. For RJA, a sig-

    nificant effect associated with the increase in adjusted

    R2, R .58, R2 .20, adjusted R .10, p .05,

    was again observed for Step 4 of the analyses with the

    addition of the parietal data. Left parietal activation,

    1.2, p .02, and right parietal hypo-activation,

    .88, p .065, were associated with 14-month

    RJA. However, follow-up analyses did not reveal ef-

    fects for parietal processes when considered in isola-

    tion from the other EEG variables. Analyses of IBR

    at 14 months yielded a significant effect for Step 1,

    R .43, p .055, with follow-up analyses indicating

    that relatively greater short distance left frontal to cen-

    tral coherence was associated with IBR at 14 months,

    r .39, p .03. Alternatively, a marginally signifi-

    cant effect on Step 1 for the analyses of RBR was also

    observed, R .41, p .07, due to an association

    between greater right hemisphere long distance frontal

    to occipital coherence, r .34, p .056.

    Only two significant effects were observed for the

    concurrent 18-month analyses, and both of these sug-

    gested a shift from 14 to 18 months in the brain activ-

    ity correlates of IJA and RJA. For the analyses of both

    IJA and RJA, marginal to significant increases in ad-

    justed R2

    were observed for Step 5 with the additionof the 18-month occipital data, R .63, R2 .20,

    adjusted R2 .09, p .06 for IJA, and R .63,

    R2 .24, adjusted R2 .10, p .05 for RJA. Fol-

    low-up analyses revealed that only left occipital hy-

    poactivation was associated with 18-month IJA, r

    .44, p .015. Alternatively, a pattern of right occipital

    hypoactivation, .60, p .01, and right central

    activation, .66, p .006, was associated with

    RJA at 18 months, R .51, adjusted R2 .21, p

    .015.

    D ISCU SSIO N

    An important feature of the results of this study was

    that individual differences in baseline measures of left

    frontal brain activity were uniquely and specifically

    related to the subsequent development of infant IJA in

    this study of normally developing infants. This obser-

    vation was consistent with the findings of Caplan et

    al. (1993) and provides further support for the hypoth-

    esized involvement of frontal process in IJA devel-

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    10/14

    334 Mundy, Card, and Fox

    opment (Mundy, 1995). Unexpectedly, though, left

    central activity was also associated with IJA devel-

    opment. Furthermore, different types of infant social

    attention coordination skills were related to different

    patterns of cortical activity. Because aspects of this

    pattern of results were not anticipated, it may be usefulto go beyond theory reviewed in the Introduction on

    the frontal processes that may be associated with IJA

    and consider theory on the early development of at-

    tention regulation more broadly conceived.

    The emergence of attention regulation in infancy

    has been described in terms of an anteriorposterior

    model of development (Posner & Petersen, 1990;

    Rothbart, Posner, & Rosicky; 1994; Stuss et al., 1995).

    The posterior component of this system is thought to

    develop before the anterior component (Rothbart et al.,

    1994). Between 3 and 6 months of age, infants become

    capable of shifting attention by inhibiting or disengag-

    ing attention from an immediate focus ( Johnson, Pos-ner, & Rothbart, 1991). The development of this ca-

    pacity is thought to reflect a posterior orienting

    attention system regulated, in part, by components of

    the parietal cortex as well as associated midbrain and

    thalamic areas (Posner & Petersen, 1990). The regu-

    lation of behavior via this system may be best thought

    of in terms of implicit learning, where this refers to

    the action of relatively less voluntary neurobehavioral

    processes (Rothbart et al., 1994).

    From this theoretical perspective, the data in this

    study suggest that responding to joint attention skill

    development may reflect the operations of this poste-riorparietal attention system to some significant ex-

    tent. The parietal lobe function of enabling shifts of

    attention by disengaging attention from its current fo-

    cus (Rothbart et al., 1994, p. 637) may be employed

    when an infant redirects his or her attention in re-

    sponse to the head and gaze shift of an experimenter

    on an RJA trial. Furthermore, RJA functions appear

    to become part of many infants behavioral repertoire

    by 6 months of age (Butterworth, 1995; Morales et al.,

    1998; Scaife & Bruner, 1975), and data also suggest

    that the parietal orienting system becomes functional

    just prior to this age, in the 4- to 6-month period (John-

    son et al., 1991).

    Other circumstantial evidence points to a link be-

    tween RJA and parietal lobe functioning. The parietal

    lobes, in conjunction with occipital functions, may

    serve to encode spatial location of stimuli relative to

    body position and eye orientation (Andersen &

    Mountcastle, 1983; Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Motter,

    Steinmetz, Duffy, & Mountcastle, 1987). This is con-

    sistent with the hypothesized importance of spatial an-

    alytic skill in the acquisition and development of RJA

    skill (Butterworth, 1995; Butterworth & Cochran,

    1980; Butterworth & Jarrett, 1991). In addition, recall

    that RJA skill has been linked to early receptive lan-

    guage development (Baldwin, 1991, 1993; Morales et

    al., 1998; Mundy & Gomes, 1998; Mundy et al.,

    1995). Similarly, cortical evoked response data sug-

    gest that the left parietal as well as the left temporalprocesses are involved in the discrimination of com-

    prehended versus noncomprehended words in the sec-

    ond year (Mills, Coffey-Corina, & Neville, 1994).

    Thus, there are numerous strands of theory and re-

    search that are consistent with the linkage observed

    here between parietal EEG and RJA.

    Alternatively, the frontal correlates of IJA, and to

    a lesser extent IBR and RBR, may reflect the influence

    and maturation of an anterior attention regulation net-

    work. This network becomes functional after the pos-

    terior parietal system, and is manifest in increasing

    volitional control of attention deployment (Rothbart et

    al., 1994). The anterior attention system plays a centralrole in the capacity to share attention across dual tasks

    or foci of attention (Stuss et al., 1995), as well as in

    the regulation of voluntary, goal-directed orienting as

    affected by both cognitive and motivational constraint

    and activation parameters (Neafsey, Terreberry, Hur-

    ley, Ruit, & Frysztak, 1993; Rothbart et al., 1994).

    Thus, consistent with theory espoused in the Introduc-

    tion, the connection of IJA to a frontal anterior atten-

    tion system may reflect cognitive, executive, and/or

    motivational processes.

    There is also some evidence that the dorsolateral

    and midfrontal cortex, including the anterior cingulategyrus, may contribute to this anterior executive atten-

    tion network (Cummings, 1995; Rothbart et al., 1994;

    Stuss et al., 1995). This area, especially the cingulate

    gyrus, has interconnections to the limbic system as

    well as the central motor areas (Rothbart et al., 1994),

    including those involved in head and eye movements

    involved in attention and orienting (Neafsey et al.,

    1993). The latter connections may provide a starting

    point in considering why an integrated pattern of 14-

    month left frontal and central cortical components was

    associated with 18-month IJA, and to a lesser degree

    14-month IJA. The results suggested that, at 14

    months, a pattern of right central deactivation and left

    frontal activation as well as a decrease in the syn-

    chrony of activation of left frontal and central cortical

    sites was related to IJA development. This pattern, es-

    pecially the latter component, suggests that IJA in the

    second year may be related to developmental brain-

    maturation processes involving the differentiation of

    left cortical functions (Thatcher, 1994). In particular,

    IJA development at 18 months may have been pre-

    saged by the differentiation and emphasis of frontal

    relative to central contributions to attention regulation.

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    11/14

    Cortical Activity and Joint Attention 335

    Alternatively, concurrent correlations at 14 months

    suggested that, although IJA was beginning to become

    associated with a frontal central differentiation, central

    components were primary to IJA at this point in de-

    velopment.

    Of course, such a rich interpretation of a single dataset may be premature. Caution should be exercised in

    interpreting this relatively unprecedented data set.

    Nevertheless, this analysis serves to facilitate a poten-

    tially edifying integration between more general as-

    pects of research on human attention processes (e.g.,

    Posner & Petersen, 1990; Rothbart et al., 1994) and

    contemporary research on infant social attention co-

    ordination (e.g., Baldwin, 1991; Baron-Cohen, 1995;

    Corkum & Moore, 1997; Mundy & Gomes, 1998)

    that, heretofore, has gone unrecognized. For example,

    the notion that IJA and RJA may, respectively, reflect

    a later developing anterior attention regulation net-

    work versus an earlier arising parietal attention regu-lation network may assist in understanding why these

    measures have been observed to display different pat-

    terns of behavioral correlates in typical and atypical

    samples (Claussen et al., 1999; Mundy & Gomes,

    1998; Sigman & Ruskin, 1999; Ulvund & Smith,

    1996).

    This dual neural-network hypothesis of joint atten-

    tion development may also have implications for un-

    derstanding the disturbance in this skill domain that is

    characteristic of autism (Mundy, 1995). Children with

    autism display deficits in both IJA and RJA, with the

    former being more robust with development than thelatter (Mundy et al., 1994; Sigman & Ruskin, 1999).

    The data in this study lead to the suggestion that both

    left frontal and parietal processes may be involved in

    this area of disturbance for children with autism. This

    notion is consistent with other research on autism. For

    example, abnormal seretonin synthesis in the left fron-

    tal hemisphere may be characteristic of a substantial

    number of children with autism (Chugani et al., 1997).

    Recent functional MRI (fMRI) data have also sug-

    gested that socialcognitive task performance may be

    associated with a left medial frontal system of activity,

    and that individual disorders related to autism may

    display a disturbance in the functions of this medial

    frontal system (Fletcher et al., 1995; Happe et al.,

    1996; for discussion see Mundy & Neal, in press).

    Indeed, the combination of results of these fMRI stud-

    ies and this study provides some support for the hy-

    pothesis that deficits observed in children with autism

    in joint attention skill as well as in later socialcog-

    nitive, or so-called Theory of Mind facility, may be

    linked in some fundamental fashion (Baron-Cohen,

    1995; Mundy & Neal, in press). Finally, studies have

    also reported observations of parietal lobe abnormal-

    ities in individuals with autism (Piven, Arndt, Bailey,

    & Andreasen, 1996), as well as individuals with the

    often-related disorder of fragile X syndrome (Schapiro

    et al., 1995).

    This dual neural-network hypothesis of joint atten-

    tion development may also prove instructive with re-gard to a prevailing notion that all forms of infant so-

    cial attention coordination primarily reflect a common

    epistemological process, such as understanding inten-

    tions in self and other (Carpenter et al., 1998; Toma-

    sello, 1995; Wellman, 1993), or a single modular sys-

    tem of socialcognitive development (Baron-Cohen,

    1995). In contrast, the relative lack of significant cor-

    relations among infant social attention coordination

    measures observed here, and the differential pattern of

    their correlations with EEG data, was consistent with

    theory that suggests that different types of infant at-

    tention coordination acts may reflect distinct constel-

    lations and integrations of neurological, psychologi-cal, and experiential processes as well as common

    processes (Mundy, 1995; Mundy & Gomes, 1997).

    Thus, in the study of the development of infant atten-

    tion coordination skills, it may be useful to consider

    the possibility that different skills reflect divergent as

    well as convergent processes in their development.

    The data also provided some support for Fischer

    and Roses (1994) notion that, at different points in

    the development of a skill, different integrations of

    frontal, central, parietal, and occipital EEG associa-

    tions may be manifest. Indeed, based on data from

    Hudspeth and Pribram (1992), Fischer and Rose sug-gested that more anterior brain functions may be in-

    volved in the early phases of skill development,

    whereas more posterior brain functions may underpin

    later phases of consolidation. The transition from con-

    current and predictive associations with a frontal/cen-

    tral system for IJA and a parietal system for RJA at

    14 months to concurrent associations with an occipital

    activity for both variables by 18 months in this study

    was consistent with this view of a complex, shifting

    pattern of cortical correlates of behavioral develop-

    ment.

    Finally, several methodological issues bear men-

    tion. First, all the parents of infants in this study were

    right-handed. Perhaps, then, the left hemisphere as-

    sociations in this study reflected a bias in infants use

    of their right hands to point or gesture. This issue war-

    rants further consideration. However, similar behav-

    iors, such as pointing, were observed in both the IJA

    and IBR measures in this study (see Table 1), but these

    measures had different patterns of cortical correlates.

    Hence, it seems unlikely that a right-handed bias could

    fully account for left hemisphere brainbehavior re-

    lations observed in this study.

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    12/14

    336 Mundy, Card, and Fox

    Second, it is important to note that the 14- to 18-

    month period of this study may be especially sensitive

    to consolidation of individual differences in IJA skills.

    If frontal functions are generally prominent during

    sensitive periods of skill development (Fischer &

    Rose, 1994), then the specific association of IJA withleft frontal EEG activation and frontal EEG coherence

    may have been observed only because of the devel-

    opmental window chosen for study. For example, the

    peak consolidation period for RJA may be somewhere

    in the 6- to 15-month period (Butterworth & Jarrett,

    1991; Morales et al., 1998; Scaife & Bruner, 1975).

    Perhaps examination of the EEG correlates of RJA in

    that period would yield a different and comparably

    complex pattern of cortical correlates.

    Finally, the IJA measure displayed the strongest

    and most consistent evidence of longitudinal stability

    or test-retest reliability in this study. This difference

    in the psychometric properties of the variables understudy could have effected the pattern of the results.

    However, the stability of the IJA measure may also

    attest to its relative importance. IJA may be a valid

    index of a variety of cognitive, temperamental, and

    environmental factors that culminate in individual dif-

    ferences in infants tendencies to self-organize and

    contribute to opportunities for social observational

    learning (Claussen et al., 1999; Mundy & Neal, in

    press; Ulvund & Smith, 1996) and these opportunities

    may be critical to fostering young childrens cognitive

    assimilation of cultural information and adaptive so-

    cial development (e.g., Adamson, 1995; Bruner, 1975;Mundy & Willoughby, 1998; Tomasello et al., 1993;

    Trevarthen, 1979; Ulvund & Smith, 1996). As such,

    IJA, and the capacity to initiate sharing of information

    with others, may be a major milestone of early devel-

    opment that serves as a behavioral fulcrum around

    which multiple domains are integrated and organized

    early in development (Mundy, 1995). Hence, the

    unique frontal component and relative complexity of

    the pattern of cortical correlates of IJA observed in

    this study may be consistent with the hypothesized

    central importance of this domain for early develop-

    ment.

    REFEREN CES

    Adamson, L. (1995). Communication development during

    infancy. Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark.

    Andersen, R., & Mountcastle, V. (1983). The influence of

    angle of gaze upon the sensitivity of light-sensitive neu-

    rons of the posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Neurosci-

    ence, 3, 532548.

    Baldwin, D. (1991). Infants contribution to the achievement

    of joint reference. Child Development, 63, 875 890.

    Baldwin, D. (1993). Early referential understanding: Infants

    ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. De-

    velopmental Psychology, 29, 832 843.

    Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Joint attention deficits in autism:

    Towards a cognitive analysis. Development and Psycho-pathology, 1, 185189.

    Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA:

    MIT Press.

    Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., & Camaioni, L. (1979).

    The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communica-

    tion in infancy. New York: Academic Press.

    Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acqui-

    sition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer

    Quarterly, 21, 205224.

    Bell, M. A., & Fox, N. A. (1992). The relations between

    frontal brain electrical activity and cognitive development

    during infancy. Child Development, 63, 1142 1163.

    Bell, M. A., & Fox, N. A. (1996). Crawling experience isrelated to changes in cortical organization during infancy:

    Evidence from EEG coherence. Developmental Psycho-

    biology, 29, 551561.

    Bruner, J. (1975). From communication to language: A psy-

    chological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255 287.

    Bruner, J., & Sherwood, V. (1983). Thought, language, and

    interaction in infancy. In J. Call, E. Galenson, & R. Tyson

    (Eds.), Frontiers of infant psychiatry (pp. 5 25). New

    York: Basic Books.

    Butterworth, G. (1995). Origins of mind in perceptions and

    action. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention:

    Its origins and role in development (pp. 29 40). Hillsdale,

    NJ: Erlbaum.

    Butterworth, G., & Cochran, E. (1980). Towards a mecha-

    nism of joint visual attention in human infancy. Interna-

    tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 3, 253 272.

    Butterworth, G., & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in

    common is space: Spatial mechanisms in serving joint

    visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Develop-

    mental Psychology, 9, 5572.

    Calkins, S. D., Fox, N. A., & Marshall, T. R. (1996). Be-

    havioral and physiological antecedents of inhibited and

    uninhibited behavior. Child Development, 67, 523 540.

    Caplan, R., Chugani, H., Messa, C., Guthrie, D., Sigman,

    M., Traversay, J., & Mundy, P. (1993). Hemispherectomy

    for early onset intractable seizures: Presurgical cerebral

    glucose metabolism and postsurgical nonverbal commu-nication patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child

    Neurology, 35, 574581.

    Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social

    cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence

    from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society

    of Research in Child Development, 63(Serial No. 4).

    Chugani, D., Muzik, O., Rothermel, R., Behen, M., Chak-

    raborty, P., Mangner, T., da Silva, E., & Chugani, H.

    (1997). Altered serotonin synthesis in the denatothlamo-

    cortical pathway in autistic boys. Annals of Neurology,

    42, 666 669.

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    13/14

    Cortical Activity and Joint Attention 337

    Claussen, A., Mundy, P., & Willoughby, J. (submitted).

    Joint attention and attachment in infants at risk. (Paper

    originally presented at the International Society on Infant

    Studies, April, 1998, Atlanta).

    Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/

    correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale,

    NJ: Erlbaum.Corkum, V., & Moore, C. (1997). The origins of joint atten-

    tion. Developmental Psychology, 34, 28 38.

    Cummings, J. (1995). Anatomic and behavioral aspects of

    frontal-subcortical circuits. In G. Jordan, K. Holyoak, et

    al. (Eds.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,

    769, (pp. 113). New York: New York Academy of Sci-

    ences.

    Diamond, A. (1988). Abilities and neural mechanisms un-

    derlying AB performance. Child Development, 59, 523

    527.

    Fischer, K., & Rose, S. (1994). Dynamic development of

    coordination of components in brain and behavior: A

    framework for theory and research. In G. Dawson & K.Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain

    (pp. 366). New York: Guilford Press.

    Fletcher, P., Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S., Sloan, R., Frac-

    kowiak, R., & Frith, C. (1995). Other minds in the brain:

    A functional imaging study of theory of mind in story

    comprehension. Cognition, 57, 109 128.

    Fox, N. (1991). Its not left, its right: Electroencephalo-

    graph asymmetry and the development of emotion. Amer-

    ican Psychologist, 46, 863872.

    Fox, N., & Davidson, R. (1987). EEG asymmetry in 10-

    month-old infants in response to approach of a stranger

    and maternal separation. Developmental Psychology, 23,

    233240.

    Griffith, E., Pennington, B., Wehner, E., & Rogers, S.

    (1999). Executive functions in children with autism. Child

    Development, 70, 817832.

    Goldman-Rakic, P. (1987). Circuitry of primate prefrontal

    cortex and regulation of behavior by representational

    memory. In B. Plum (Ed.), The handbook of physiology,

    the nervous system, and higher brain functions (pp. 373

    417). Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society.

    Happe, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johansson, M.,

    Gillberg, C., Dolan, R., Frackowiak, R., & Frith, C.

    (1996). Theory of mind in the brain. Evidence from a PET

    scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport: An Inter-

    national Journal for the Rapid Communication of Re-

    search in Neuroscience, 8, 197201.Hudspeth, W., & Pribram, K. (1992). Psychophysiological

    indices of cerebral maturation. International Journal of

    Psychophyiosology, 12, 19 29.

    Jasper, H. (1958). The 10/20 international electrode system.

    EEG and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371375.

    Johnson, M., Posner, M., & Rothbart, M. (1991). Compo-

    nents of visual orienting in early infancy: Contingency

    learning, anticipatory looking, and disengaging. Journal

    of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 335 344.

    McEvoy, R., Rogers, S., & Pennington, R. (1993). Executive

    function and social communication deficits in young au-

    tistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-

    atry, 34, 563578.

    Mills, D., Coffey-Corina, S., & Neville, H. (1994). Varia-

    bility in cerebral organization during primary language

    acquisition. In G. Dawson & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human

    behavior and the developing brain (pp. 427455). New

    York: Guilford Press.Morales, M., Mundy, P., & Rojas, J. (1998). Gaze following

    and language development in 6-month-olds. Infant Be-

    havior and Development, 21, 373377.

    Motter, B., Steinmetz, M., Duffy, C., & Mountcastle, V.

    (1987). Functional properties of parietal visual neurons:

    Mechanism of directionality along a single axis. Journal

    of Neuroscience, 7, 154176.

    Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention and socialemotional ap-

    proach behavior in children with autism. Development

    and Psychopathology, 7, 6382.

    Mundy, P., & Gomes, A. (1997). A skills approach to com-

    munication development: Lessons learned from research

    with children with developmental disabilities. In L. Ad-amson & M. Romski (Eds.), Research on communication

    and language disorders (pp.107132). New York: Paul

    Brookes.

    Mundy, P., & Gomes, A. (1998). Individual differences in

    joint attention skill development in the second year. Infant

    Behavior and Development, 21, 469482.

    Mundy, P., Hogan, A., & Doehring, P. (1996). A preliminary

    manual for the abridged Early Social-Communication

    Scales. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami,

    www.psy.miami.edu/ faculty/pmundy.

    Mundy, P., Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1992). Joint attention,

    affective sharing, and intersubjectivity. Infant Behavior

    and Development, 15, 377381.

    Mundy, P., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1995).

    Nonverbal communication and early language in Down

    syndrome and in normally developing children. Journal

    of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 157167.

    Mundy, P., & Neal, R. (in press). Neural plasticity, joint

    attention, and autistic developmental pathology. In L.

    Glidden (Ed.), International Review of Mental Retarda-

    tion Research, Vol. 27.

    Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention,

    developmental level, and symptom presentation in autism.

    Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389 402.

    Mundy, P., & Willoughby, J. (1998). Nonverbal communi-

    cation, affect, and social emotional development. In A.

    Wetherby, S. Warren, & J. Reichle (Eds.), Transitions inprelinguistic communication: Preintentional to intentional

    and presymbolic to symbolic (pp. 111134). Baltimore,

    MD: Brookes.

    Neafsey, E., Terreberry, R., Hurley, K., Ruit, K., & Frysztak,

    R. (1993). Anterior cingulate cortex of rodents: Connec-

    tions, visceral control functions, and implications for

    emotion. In B. Vogt & M. Gabriel (Eds.), Neurobiology

    of the cingulate cortex and limbic thalamus: A Compre-

    hensive handbook (pp. 206 223). Boston: Birkhauser.

    Parmelee, A., Sigman, M., Garbanati, J., Cohen, S., Beck-

    with, L., & Asarnow, R. (1994). Neonatal electroenceph-

  • 7/29/2019 05cercetare

    14/14

    338 Mundy, Card, and Fox

    alographic organization and attention in early adoles-

    cence. In G. Dawson & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human

    behavior and the developing brain (pp. 537554). New

    York: Guilford Press.

    Piven, J., Arndt, S., Bailey, J., & Adreasen, N. (1996). Re-

    gional brain enlargement in autism: A magnetic resonance

    imaging study. Journal of the American Academy ofChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 530536.

    Posner, M., & Petersen, S. (1990). The attention system of

    the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13,

    2542.

    Rothbart, M., Posner, M., & Rosicky, J. (1994). Orienting

    in normal and pathological development. Development

    and Psychopathology, 6, 635652.

    Saltzberg, B., Burton, D. B., Burch, N. R., Fletcher, J., &

    Michaels, R. (1986). Electro-physiological measures of

    regional neural interactive coupling. Linear and nonlinear

    dependence relationships among multiple channel electro-

    encephalographic recordings. International Journal of

    Bio-Medical Computing, 18, 77 87.

    Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. (1975). The capacity for joint visual

    attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265266.

    Schapiro, M., Murphy, D., Hagerman, R., Azari, N., Alex-

    ander, G., Miezejeski, C., Hinton, V., Horowitz, B.,

    Haxby, J., & Kumar, A. (1995). Adult fragile X syn-

    drome: Neuropsychology, brain anatomy, and metabo-

    lism. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 60, 480

    493.

    Seibert, J. M., Hogan, A. E., & Mundy, P. C. (1982). As-

    sessing interactional competencies: The early social-com-

    munication scales. Infant Mental Health Journal, 3, 244

    245.

    Sheinkopf, S., Mundy, P., Willoughby, J., & Claussen. A.

    (1999, April). Joint attention in cocaine infants and 36-month behavioral outcome. Paper presented at the meet-

    ing of the Society for Research in Child Development,

    Albuquerque, NM.

    Sigman, M. & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in

    the social competence of children with autism, Down

    Syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of

    the Society for Research in Child Development, 64,

    Vol. 114.

    Stuss, D., Shallice, T., Alexander, M., & Picton, T. (1995).

    A multidisciplinary approach to anterior attentional func-

    tions. In G. Jordan, K. Holyoak, & Boller, F. (Eds.), An-

    nals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, (pp.191211). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

    Thatcher, R. (1994). Cyclical cortical reorganization: Ori-

    gins of human cognitive development. In G. Dawson &

    K. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing

    brain (pp. 232268). New York: Guilford Press.

    Thatcher, R., Krause, P., & Hrybyk, M. (1986). Cortico-

    cortical association fibers and EEG coherence: A two-

    compartmental model. Electroencephalography and Clin-

    ical Neurophy, 64, 123143.

    Thorpe, S., Rolls, E., & Maddison, S. (1983). The orbitof-

    rontal cortex: Neuronal activity in the behaving monkey.

    Experimental Brain Research, 49, 93 115.

    Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In

    C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins

    and role in development (pp. 103130). Hillsdale, NJ:

    Erlbaum.

    Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, A. (1993). Cultural

    learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495552.

    Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in

    infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M.

    Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human

    communication (pp. 321347). London: Cambridge Uni-

    versity Press.

    Ulvund, S., & Smith, L. (1996). The predictive validity of

    nonverbal communication skills in infants with perinatal

    hazards. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 441449.

    Wellman, H. (1993). Early understanding of mind: The nor-mal case. In S. Baron-Cohen, H., Tager-Flusberg, & D.

    Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds (pp. 1039).

    Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Werner, H., & Kaplan, S. (1963). Symbol formation. New

    York: Wiley.